Skip to main content
Social Sci LibreTexts

8.3: Perception and Action

  • Page ID
    134398
  • This page is a draft and under active development. Please forward any questions, comments, and/or feedback to the ASCCC OERI (oeri@asccc.org).

    \( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \) \( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)\(\newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\) \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\) \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \(\newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\) \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\) \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)\(\newcommand{\AA}{\unicode[.8,0]{x212B}}\)

    Learning Objectives
    • Explain how perception guides action with an example
    • Describe how action changes perception with an example
    • Understand that acting on unconscious sensory information is possible

    Brief Overview

    What is the point of perceiving things if we cannot interact with them in some way? Perception is rarely a passive experience; it is almost always shaped by some kind of goal. Our perception is also shaped by the fact that it simply isn't feasible to process our entire sensory experience. We selectively attend to subsets of our experience and process that; much of our sensory world is not encoded. What we attend to depends on our goals at the moment, so our actions shape our perception and our perception shapes our action.

    The relationship between perception and action is best described by a diagram with an arrow pointing both ways. Perception selects targets for action and helps us correct errors as we execute actions. Broadly speaking, there are two kinds of actions: navigation (moving around our environment) and intentional movements (such as reaching/grabbing) Fig 8.3.1.

    brainsci-10-00174-g001-550-300x117.jpg
    Fig 8.3.1 This figure shows the relationship between perception and action, which is described as a cycle of arrows pointing back and forth. An example of this cycle is when you reach out for a bottle sitting on a slanted surface: Banks et al (2000) showed that your sense of touch updates your visual estimates of the surface slant. (Provided by: MDPI. License: CC-BY 4.0)

    Acting on unconscious sensory information is possible. There are a few dramatic examples of action without perception. For example a patient, DF, could mail letters through a slot but couldn’t tell you if the slot had a horizontal or vertical orientation, or patient TN, who was completely blind but could navigate cluttered hallways. TN’s case makes it obvious that visual information reaches the non-visual regions of the brain (such as areas of the parietal cortex) even when the primary visual cortex (V1) is not functioning typically.

    The Ebbinghaus size illusion Fig 8.3.2is an optical illusion of relative size perception. In the best-known version of the illusion, two circles of identical size are placed near to each other, and one is surrounded by large circles while the other is surrounded by small circles. As a result of the juxtaposition of circles, the central circle surrounded by large circles appears smaller than the central circle surrounded by small circles.

    1920px-Mond-vergleich.svg (1).png

    Fig 8.3.2 The two orange central circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the right appears larger.

    Similarly, healthy controls accurately grasp a center object affected by the rod-and-frame illusion, which is the phenomenon that the orientation of a line is altered by surrounding lines or grating (Dyde, 2000). These studies suggest that all of us maintain a “raw copy” of sensory information, separate from the version that we’re consciously aware of (which has been shaped by inference), to guide action. However, there is contradictory research indicating that motor planning (Craje, 2008) and reaching behaviors (Dyde, 2000) can be impacted by illusions. So it remains an open and interesting question: When is our behavior influenced by illusions, and when does control of our motor system ignore illusions?

    Attribution

    References

    Crajé, C., van der Kamp, J., & Steenbergen, B. (2008). The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task. Experimental brain research, 185(1), 53–62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1130-x

    Dyde RT, Milner AD (2002) Two illusions of perceived orientations: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time. Exp Brain Res 144:518–527

    Ernst, M., Banks, M. & Bülthoff, H. Touch can change visual slant perception. Nat Neurosci 3, 69–73 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1038/71140

    Haffenden AM, Goodale MA. The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10(1):122-136. doi:10.1162/089892998563824


    This page titled 8.3: Perception and Action is shared under a mixed license and was authored, remixed, and/or curated by Multiple Authors (ASCCC Open Educational Resources Initiative (OERI)) .