Skip to main content
Social Sci LibreTexts

8.8: References

  • Page ID
    140009
    • Todd LaMarr
    \( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \) \( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)\(\newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\) \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\) \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \(\newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\) \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\) \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)\(\newcommand{\AA}{\unicode[.8,0]{x212B}}\)

    References for Overview of Cognitive Development

    • Aditomo, A. (2015). Students' response to academic setback: "Growth mindset" as a buffer against demotivation. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 4(2), 198-222.
    • Aslin, R. N. (2007). What's in a look? Developmental Science, 10(1), 48-53.
    • Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½-and 4½-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 655.
    • Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants' physical world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 89-94.
    • Bahrick, L. E., Todd, J. T., Castellanos, I., & Sorondo, B. M. (2016). Enhanced attention to speaking faces versus other event types emerges gradually across infancy. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1705.
    • Balaban, M. T., & Waxman, S. R. (1997). Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64(1), 3-26.
    • Banerjee, P. N., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2007). Infants’ persistence and mothers’ teaching as predictors of toddlers’ cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(3), 479-491.
    • Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (2003). It's not what you know, it's who you know: Older siblings facilitate imitation during infancy. International Journal of Early Years Education, 11(1), 7-21.
    • Beilin, H., & Fireman, G. (1999). The foundation of Piaget's theories: Mental and physical action. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 27, 221-246.
    • Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self‐regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326-339.
    • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246-263.
    • Bohlmann, N. L., & Downer, J. T. (2016). Self-regulation and task engagement as predictors of emergent language and literacy skills. Early Education and Development, 27(1), 18-37.
    • Bornstein, M. H., & Sigman, M. D. (1986). Continuity in mental development from infancy. Child Development, 251-274.
    • Bourke, L., Davies, S. J., Sumner, E., & Green, C. (2014). Individual differences in the development of early writing skills: Testing the unique contribution of visuo-spatial working memory. Reading and Writing, 27(2), 315-335.
    • Bradley, R. H., McKelvey, L. M., & Whiteside‐Mansell, L. (2011). Does the quality of stimulation and support in the home environment moderate the effect of early education programs? Child Development, 82(6), 2110-2122.
    • Bremner, J. G., Slater, A. M., & Johnson, S. P. (2015). Perception of object persistence: The origins of object permanence in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 9(1), 7-13.
    • Brummelman, E., Thomaes, S., Overbeek, G., Orobio de Castro, B., Van Den Hout, M. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). On feeding those hungry for praise: Person praise backfires in children with low self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 9.
    • Calkins, S.D. (2007). The emergence of self-regulation: Biological and behavioral control mechanisms supporting toddler competencies. In Socioemotional Development in the Toddler Years: Transitions and Transformations; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA.
    • Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14(3), 477-498.
    • Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149-219.
    • Carey, S. (2009). Where our number concepts come from. The Journal of Philosophy, 106(4), 220.
    • Carlson, S. M. (2003). Executive function in context: Development, measurement, theory, and experience. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), 138-151.
    • Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 595–616.
    • Casey, B. M., Pezaris, E., Fineman, B., Pollock, A., Demers, L., & Dearing, E. (2015). A longitudinal analysis of early spatial skills compared to arithmetic and verbal skills as predictors of fifth-grade girls' math reasoning. Learning and Individual Differences, 40, 90-100.
    • Caughy, M. O. B., Mills, B., Owen, M. T., & Hurst, J. R. (2013). Emergent self-regulation skills among very young ethnic minority children: A confirmatory factor model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4), 839–855.
    • Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: a cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 677.
    • ​​Cheng, N., Lu, S., Archer, M., & Wang, Z. (2018). Quality of maternal parenting of 9-month-old infants predicts executive function performance at 2 and 3 years of age. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2293.
    • Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(35), 12629-12633.
    • Colombo, J. (1993). Infant cognition (Vol. 5). Sage.
    • Colombo, J., & Mitchell, D. W. (2009). Infant visual habituation. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 92(2), 225-234.
    • Constantinescu, M., Moore, D. S., Johnson, S. P., & Hines, M. (2018). Early contributions to infants’ mental rotation abilities. Developmental Science, 21(4), e12613.
    • Cornu, V., Schiltz, C., Martin, R., & Hornung, C. (2018). Visuo-spatial abilities are key for young children’s verbal number skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 604-620.
    • Côté, S. M., Mongeau, C., Japel, C., Xu, Q., Séguin, J. R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2013). Child care quality and cognitive development: Trajectories leading to better preacademic skills. Child Development, 84(2), 752-766.
    • Dawkins, M. B., Sloane, S., & Baillargeon, R. (2019). Do infants in the first year of life expect equal resource allocations? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 116.
    • Daneri, M. P., Blair, C., Kuhn, L. J., & FLP Key Investigators (2019). Maternal language and child vocabulary mediate relations between socioeconomic status and executive function during early childhood. Child Development, 90(6), 2001-2018.
    • Dewar, K., & Xu, F. (2007). Do 9-month-old infants expect distinct words to refer to kinds? Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1227.
    • Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 335–341.
    • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
    • Diamond, A., Kirkham, N., & Amso, D. (2002). Conditions under which young children can hold two rules in mind and inhibit a prepotent response. Developmental Psychology, 38, 352–362.
    • Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 1087.
    • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A word from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6(4), 267-285.
    • Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York, NY: Random House.
    • Dweck, C. S. (2008). Mindsets: How praise is harming youth and what can be done about it. School Library Media Activities Monthly, 24(5), 55.
    • Ellefson, M. R., Ng, F. F. Y., Wang, Q., & Hughes, C. (2017). Efficiency of executive function: a two-generation cross-cultural comparison of samples from Hong Kong and the United Kingdom. Psychological Science, 28(5), 555-566.
    • Eskreis-Winkler, L., Duckworth, A. L., Shulman, E. P., & Beal, S. (2014). The grit effect: Predicting retention in the military, the workplace, school and marriage. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 36.
    • Fanari, R., Meloni, C., & Massidda, D. (2019). Visual and spatial working memory abilities predict early math skills: a longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2460.
    • Fay‐Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Development Perspectives, 8(4), 258-264.
    • Feigenson, L. (2011). Predicting sights from sounds: 6-month-olds’ intermodal numerical abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(3), 347-361.
    • Feigenson, L., Libertus, M. E., & Halberda, J. (2013). Links between the intuitive sense of number and formal mathematics ability. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 74-79.
    • Ferry, A. L., Hespos, S. J., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). Categorization in 3‐and 4‐month‐old infants: an advantage of words over tones. Child Development, 81(2), 472-479.
    • Ferry, A. L., Hespos, S. J., & Waxman, S. R. (2013). Nonhuman primate vocalizations support categorization in very young human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(38), 15231-15235.
    • Fiske, A., & Holmboe, K. (2019). Neural substrates of early executive function development. Developmental Review, 52, 42-62.
    • Forget‐Dubois, N., Dionne, G., Lemelin, J. P., Pérusse, D., Tremblay, R. E., & Boivin, M. (2009). Early child language mediates the relation between home environment and school readiness. Child Development, 80(3), 736-749.
    • Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., & Haltigan, J. D. (2013). The legacy of early experiences in development: formalizing alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 109.
    • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186–204.
    • Fulkerson, A. L., & Waxman, S. R. (2007). Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization: Evidence from 6-and 12-month-olds. Cognition, 105(1), 218-228.
    • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31.
    • Gerhard-Samunda, T. M., Jovanovic, B., & Schwarzer, G. (2021). Role of manually-generated visual cues in crawling and non-crawling 9-month-old infants’ mental rotation. Cognitive Development, 59, 101053.
    • Goertz, C., Lamm, B., Graf, F., Kolling, T., Knopf, M., & Keller, H. (2011). Deferred imitation in 6-month-old German and Cameroonian Nso infants. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 10(1), 44-55.
    • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415(6873), 755-755.
    • Grabell, A. S., Olson, S. L., Miller, A. L., Kessler, D. A., Felt, B., Kaciroti, N., ... & Tardif, T. (2015). The impact of culture on physiological processes of emotion regulation: A comparison of US and Chinese preschoolers. Developmental Science, 18(3), 420-435.
    • Gunderson, E. A., Gripshover, S. J., Romero, C., Dweck, C. S., Goldin‐Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2013). Parent praise to 1‐to 3‐year‐olds predicts children's motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Development, 84(5), 1526-1541.
    • Gunderson, E. A., Sorhagen, N. S., Gripshover, S. J., Dweck, C. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2018). Parent praise to toddlers predicts fourth grade academic achievement via children’s incremental mindsets. Developmental Psychology, 54(3), 397.
    • Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the" Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1457.
    • Hammond, S. I., Müller, U., Carpendale, J. I., Bibok, M. B., & Liebermann-Finestone, D. P. (2012). The effects of parental scaffolding on preschoolers' executive function. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 271.
    • Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Can instructional and emotional support in the first-grade classroom make a difference for children at risk of school failure? Child Development, 76, 949–967.
    • Helmerhorst, K. O., Colonnesi, C., & Fukkink, R. G. (2019). Caregiver’s mind-mindedness in early center-based childcare. Early Education and Development, 30(7), 854-871.
    • Hendry, A., & Holmboe, K. (2021). Development and validation of the Early Executive Functions Questionnaire: A carer‐administered measure of executive functions suitable for 9‐to 30‐month‐olds. Infancy.
    • Hendry, A., Jones, E. J. H., & Charman, T. (2016). Executive function in the first three years of life: Precursors, predictors and patterns. Developmental Review, 42, 1– 33.
    • Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 37–45.
    • Hespos, S. J., & Rochat, P. (1997). Dynamic mental representation in infancy. Cognition, 64(2), 153-188.
    • Hughes, C., Devine, R. T., & Wang, Z. (2018). Does parental mind‐mindedness account for cross‐cultural differences in preschoolers’ theory of mind? Child Development, 89(4), 1296-1310.
    • Hughes, C. H., & Ensor, R. A. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2009(123), 35-50.
    • Izard, V., Sann, C., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A. (2009). Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(25), 10382-10385.
    • Jackson, I., & Sirois, S. (2009). Infant cognition: going full factorial with pupil dilation. Developmental Science, 12(4), 670-679.
    • James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    • Jansen, P., & Heil, M. (2010). The relation between motor development and mental rotation ability in 5-to 6-year-old children. International Journal of Developmental Science, 4(1), 67-75.
    • Jaramillo, J. M., Rendón, M. I., Muñoz, L., Weis, M., & Trommsdorff, G. (2017). Children’s self-regulation in cultural contexts: The role of parental socialization theories, goals, and practices. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 923.
    • Jian, H. (2009). A Contrastive study of cultural diversity of learning styles between China and the United States. International Education Studies, 2(1), 163-166.
    • Johnson, S. P., & Moore, D. S. (2020). Spatial thinking in infancy: Origins and development of mental rotation between 3 and 10 months of age. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 5(1), 1-14.
    • Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213-233.
    • Justice, L. M., Purtell, K. M., Bleses, D., & Cho, S. (2020). Parents’ growth mindsets and home-learning activities: A cross-cultural comparison of Danish and U.S. parents. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1365.
    • Kamins, M. L., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Person versus process praise and criticism: implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Developmental Psychology, 35(3), 835.
    • Kennedy-Costantini, S., Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Davis, J., ... & Slaughter, V. (2017). There is no compelling evidence that human neonates imitate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40.
    • Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.
    • Köster, M., Langeloh, M., & Hoehl, S. (2019). Visually entrained theta oscillations increase for unexpected events in the infant brain. Psychological Science, 30(11), 1656-1663.
    • Kuchirko, Y., & Nayfeld, I. (2020). Language gap: Cultural assumptions and ideologies. In International approaches to bridging the language gap (pp. 32-53). IGI Global.
    • Kuchirko, Y. A., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2019). The cultural context of infant development: Variability, specificity, and universality. In Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 57, pp. 27-63). JAI.
    • Kugiumutzakis, G., & Trevarthen, C. (2015). Neonatal imitation. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 16, 481-488.
    • Ladd, G. W., Birch, S. H., & Buhs, E. S. (1999). Children's social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related spheres of influence? Child Development, 70, 1373–1400.
    • Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: A cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 677-692.
    • Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children's development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 15-41.
    • Lauer, J. E., Udelson, H. B., Jeon, S. O., & Lourenco, S. F. (2015). An early sex difference in the relation between mental rotation and object preference. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 558.
    • Lehmann, J., Quaiser-Pohl, C., & Jansen, P. (2014). Correlation of motor skill, mental rotation, and working memory in 3-to 6-year-old children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(5), 560-573.
    • Leonard, J. A., Lee, Y., & Schulz, L. E. (2017). Infants make more attempts to achieve a goal when they see adults persist. Science, 357(6357), 1290-1294.
    • Lewis, C., Koyasu, M., Oh, S., Ogawa, A., Short, B., & Huang, Z. (2009). Culture, executive function, and social understanding. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 69-85.
    • Liben, L. S., Myers, L. J., Christensen, A. E., & Bower, C. A. (2013). Environmental‐Scale map use in middle childhood: Links to spatial skills, strategies, and gender. Child Development, 84(6), 2047-2063.
    • Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2013). Is approximate number precision a stable predictor of math ability?. Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 126-133.
    • Lin, B., Coburn, S. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2016). Self-regulation and reading achievement. In The cognitive development of reading and reading comprehension (pp. 75-94). Routledge.
    • Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Origins of number sense: Large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science, 14(5), 396-401.
    • Lucca, K., Horton, R., & Sommerville, J. A. (2019). Keep trying!: Parental language predicts infants’ persistence. Cognition, 193, 104025.
    • Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9-F16.
    • Matte-Gagné, C., & Bernier, A. (2011). Prospective relations between maternal autonomy support and child executive functioning: Investigating the mediating role of child language ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(4), 611-625.
    • Mazzocco, M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschoolers' precision of the approximate number system predicts later school mathematics performance. PLoS One, 6(9), e23749.
    • McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Duncan, R., Bowles, R. P., Acock, A. C., Miao, A., & Pratt, M. E. (2014). Predictors of early growth in academic achievement: The head-toes-knees-shoulders task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 599.
    • McCormick, B. J., Caulfield, L. E., Richard, S. A., Pendergast, L., Seidman, J. C., Maphula, A., Koshy, B., Blacy, L., Roshan, R., Baitun, N., Shrestha, R., Rasheed, M., Svensen, S., Rasmussen, Z., Scharf, R., Haque, S., Oria, R., & Murray-Kolb, L. E. (2020). Early life experiences and trajectories of cognitive development. Pediatrics, 146(3).
    • Meier, G., & Albrecht, M. H. (2003). The persistence process: Development of a stage model for goal-directed behavior. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 10(2), 43-54.
    • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers’ comments on infants’ mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(5), 637–648.
    • Meltzoff, A. N., & Marshall, P. J. (2018). Human infant imitation as a social survival circuit. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 24, 130-136.
    • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75-78.
    • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 702-709.
    • Meltzoff, A. N., Murray, L., Simpson, E., Heimann, M., Nagy, E., Nadel, J., Pedersen, E. J., Brooks, R., Messinger, D., De Pascalis, L., Subiaul, F., Paukner, A., & Ferrari, P. F. (2018). Re‐examination of Oostenbroek et al.(2016): Evidence for neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion. Developmental Science, 21(4), e12609.
    • Miller, D. J., Spiridigliozzi, G., Ryan, E. B., Callan, M. P., & Mclaughlin, J. E. (1980). Habituation and cognitive performance: Relationships between measures at four years of age and earlier assessments. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 3(2), 131-146.
    • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organisation of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14.
    • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
    • Möhring, W., & Frick, A. (2013). Touching up mental rotation: Effects of manual experience on 6‐month‐old infants’ mental object rotation. Child Development, 84(5), 1554-1565.
    • Mokrova, I. L., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., & Marcovitch, S. (2013). The role of persistence at preschool age in academic skills at kindergarten. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28(4), 1495-1503.
    • Monroy, C., Shafto, C., Castellanos, I., Bergeson, T., & Houston, D. (2019). Visual habituation in deaf and hearing infants. PloS One, 14(2), e0209265.
    • Moore, D. S., & Johnson, S. P. (2011). Mental rotation of dynamic, three‐dimensional stimuli by 3‐month‐old infants. Infancy, 16(4), 435-445.
    • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134-140.
    • Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children's motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 33.
    • Newcombe, N. S., & Frick, A. (2010). Early education for spatial intelligence: Why, what, and how. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4(3), 102-111.
    • Newcombe, N. S., and Shipley, T. F. (2015). Thinking about spatial thinking: New typology, new assessments, in Studying Visual and Spatial Reasoning for Design Creativity, (New York, NY: Springer), 179–192.
    • Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291.
    • Novack, M. A., Brentari, D., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Waxman, S. (2021). Sign language, like spoken language, promotes object categorization in young hearing infants. Cognition, 215, 104845.
    • Oakes, L. M. (2010). Using habituation of looking time to assess mental processes in infancy. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(3), 255-268.
    • Oh, S., & Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 80-99.
    • Oostenbroek, J., Redshaw, J., Davis, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Nielsen, M., Slaughter, V., & Suddendorf, T. (2018). Re-evaluating the neonatal imitation hypothesis. Developmental Science, 22(2), e12720.
    • Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., Clark, S., & Slaughter, V. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26(10), 1334-1338.
    • Pederson, D. R., Moran, G., Sitko, C., Campbell, K., Ghesquire, K., & Acton, H. (1990). Maternal sensitivity and the security of infant‐mother attachment: AQ‐sort study. Child Development, 61(6), 1974-1983.
    • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2002). What causes 3‐year‐olds' difficulty on the dimensional change card sorting task? Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 11(2), 93-105.
    • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York, Basic Books.
    • Pomerantz, E. M., & Kempner, S. G. (2013). Mothers’ daily person and process praise: Implications for children’s theory of intelligence and motivation. Developmental Psychology, 49(11), 2040.
    • Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. S., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 605.
    • Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 29(3), 386– 401.
    • Quinn, P. C., Eimas, P. D., & Tarr, M. J. (2001). Perceptual categorization of cat and dog silhouettes by 3-to 4-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental child Psychology, 79(1), 78-94.
    • Quinn, P. C., & Liben, L. S. (2014). A sex difference in mental rotation in infants: Convergent evidence. Infancy, 19(1), 103-116.
    • Radovanovic, M., Solby, H., Soldovieri, A., & Sommerville, J. (2021, preprint). Try smarter, not harder: Exploration and strategy diversity are related to infant persistence.
    • Reid, V. M., Hoehl, S., Grigutsch, M., Groendahl, A., Parise, E., & Striano, T. (2009). The neural correlates of infant and adult goal prediction: evidence for semantic processing systems. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 620.
    • Rennie, D. A. C., Bull, R., & Diamond, A. (2004). Executive functioning in preschoolers: Reducing the inhibitory demands of the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 423–443.
    • Rittle-Johnson, B., Zippert, E. L., & Boice, K. L. (2019). The roles of patterning and spatial skills in early mathematics development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 166-178.
    • Robson, D. A., Allen, M. S., & Howard, S. J. (2020). Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 146(4), 324.
    • Rochat, P., & Hespos, S. J. (1996). Tracking and anticipation of invisible spatial transformations by 4-to 8-month-old infants. Cognitive Development, 11(1), 3-17.
    • Rodriguez, E. T., & Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S. (2011). Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: Associations with children’s vocabulary and literacy skills at prekindergarten. Child Development, 82(4), 1058-1075.
    • Rome-Flanders, T., Cronk, C., & Gourde, C. (1995). Maternal scaffolding in mother-infant games and its relationship to language development: a longitudinal study. First Language, 15(45), 339-355.
    • Rowe, M. L., & Leech, K. A. (2019). A parent intervention with a growth mindset approach improves children's early gesture and vocabulary development. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12792.
    • Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and US preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 74-81.
    • Saffran, J. R., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2018). Infant statistical learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 181-203.
    • Salminen, J., Guedes, C., Lerkkanen, M. K., Pakarinen, E., & Cadima, J. (2021). Teacher–child interaction quality and children's self‐regulation in toddler classrooms in Finland and Portugal. Infant and Child Development, e2222.
    • Samdan, G., Kiel, N., Petermann, F., Rothenfußer, S., Zierul, C., & Reinelt, T. (2020). The relationship between parental behavior and infant regulation: a systematic review. Developmental Review, 57, 100923.
    • Schwarzer, G., Freitag, C., Buckel, R., & Lofruthe, A. (2013). Crawling is associated with mental rotation ability by 9‐month‐old infants. Infancy, 18(3), 432-441.
    • Schwier, C., van Maanen, C., Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., (2006). Rational Imitation in 12‐Month‐Old Infants. Infancy, 10(3), 303-311.
    • Sethi, A., Mischel, W., Aber, J. L., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (2000). The role of strategic attention deployment in development of self-regulation: Predicting preschoolers' delay of gratification from mother–toddler interactions. Developmental Psychology, 36(6), 767.
    • Simon, T. J., Hespos, S. J., & Rochat, P. (1995). Do infants understand simple arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992). Cognitive Development, 10(2), 253-269.
    • Sisk, V. F., Burgoyne, A. P., Sun, J., Butler, J. L., & Macnamara, B. N. (2018). To what extent and under which circumstances are growth mind-sets important to academic achievement? Two meta-analyses. Psychological Science, 29(4), 549-571.
    • Slaughter, V. (2021). Do Newborns Have the Ability to Imitate?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
    • Sommerville, J. A., Enright, E. A., Horton, R. O., Lucca, K., Sitch, M. J., & Kirchner-Adelhart, S. (2018). Infants’ prosocial behavior is governed by cost-benefit analyses. Cognition, 177, 12-20.
    • Song, H., & Jinyu, A. (2017). The relationship between executive functions and IQ in Korean children and the comparison with Chinese children. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 6(2), 158-165.
    • Soska, K. C., Adolph, K. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Systems in development: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 129.
    • Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99(4), 605.
    • Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 89–96.
    • Starr, A., Libertus, M. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(45), 18116-18120.
    • Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2019). Violations of core knowledge shape early learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11(1), 136-153.
    • Thompson, R. A. (2016). What more has been learned? The science of early childhood development 15 years after neurons to neighborhoods. Zero to Three Journal, 36(3), 18-24.
    • Tsujimoto, S. (2008). The prefrontal cortex: Functional neural development during early childhood. The Neuroscientist, 14(4), 345-358.
    • von Salisch, M., Haenel, M., & Denham, S. A. (2015). Self-regulation, language skills, and emotion knowledge in young children from Northern Germany. Early Education and Development, 26(5-6), 792-806.
    • Wang, J., & Feigenson, L. (2021). Dynamic changes in numerical acuity in 4‐month‐old infants. Infancy, 26(1), 47-62.
    • Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., Ponitz, C. C., Son, S. H., Lan, X., ... & Li, S. (2011). Measuring behavioral regulation in four societies. Psychological Assessment, 23(2), 364.
    • Waxman, S. R., & Braun, I. (2005). Consistent (but not variable) names as invitations to form object categories: New evidence from 12-month-old infants. Cognition, 95(3), B59-B68.
    • Waxman, S. R., Fu, X., Ferguson, B., Geraghty, K., Leddon, E., Liang, J., & Zhao, M. F. (2016). How early is infants' attention to objects and actions shaped by culture? New evidence from 24-month-olds raised in the US and China. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 97.
    • Woodruff Carr, K., Perszyk, D. R., & Waxman, S. R. (2021). Birdsong fails to support object categorization in human infants. Plos One, 16(3), e0247430.
    • Wynn, K. (1990). Children's understanding of counting. Cognition, 36(2), 155-193.
    • Wynn, K. (1992). Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 220-251.
    • Xu, F., & Arriaga, R. I. (2007). Number discrimination in 10‐month‐old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 103-108.
    • Xu, F., Cote, M., & Baker, A. (2005). Labeling guides object individuation in 12-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16(5), 372-377.
    • Xu, F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition, 74(1), B1-B11.
    • Yeager, D. S., Hanselman, P., Walton, G. M., Murray, J. S., Crosnoe, R., Muller, C., Tipton, C., Schneider, B., Hulleman, C. S., Hinojosa, C. P., Paunesku, D., Romero, C., Flint, K., Roberts, A., Trott, J., Iachan, R., Buontempo, J., Yang, S. M., Carvalho, C. M., Hahn, P. R., Gopalan, M., Mhatre, P., Ferguson, R., Duckworth A. L., & Dweck, C. S. (2019). A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature, 573(7774), 364-369.
    • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11(1), 37-63.
    • Zeng, G., Hou, H., & Peng, K. (2016). Effect of growth mindset on school engagement and psychological well-being of Chinese primary and middle school students: The mediating role of resilience. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1873.
    • Zhang, X. (2016). Linking language, visual-spatial, and executive function skills to number competence in very young Chinese children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 178-189.
    • Zmyj, N., Daum, M. M., & Aschersleben, G. (2009). The development of rational imitation in 9‐and 12‐month‐old infants. Infancy, 14(1), 131-141.

    This page titled 8.8: References is shared under a mixed 4.0 license and was authored, remixed, and/or curated by Todd LaMarr.