Skip to main content
Social Sci LibreTexts

5.5: Interbrain Neural Synchrony

  • Page ID
    198004
  • \( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

    \( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

    ( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)

    \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)

    \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\)

    \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)

    \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)

    \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\AA}{\unicode[.8,0]{x212B}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorA}[1]{\vec{#1}}      % arrow\)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorAt}[1]{\vec{\text{#1}}}      % arrow\)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorB}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorC}[1]{\textbf{#1}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorD}[1]{\overrightarrow{#1}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorDt}[1]{\overrightarrow{\text{#1}}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectE}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash{\mathbf {#1}}}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

    \( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)

    \(\newcommand{\avec}{\mathbf a}\) \(\newcommand{\bvec}{\mathbf b}\) \(\newcommand{\cvec}{\mathbf c}\) \(\newcommand{\dvec}{\mathbf d}\) \(\newcommand{\dtil}{\widetilde{\mathbf d}}\) \(\newcommand{\evec}{\mathbf e}\) \(\newcommand{\fvec}{\mathbf f}\) \(\newcommand{\nvec}{\mathbf n}\) \(\newcommand{\pvec}{\mathbf p}\) \(\newcommand{\qvec}{\mathbf q}\) \(\newcommand{\svec}{\mathbf s}\) \(\newcommand{\tvec}{\mathbf t}\) \(\newcommand{\uvec}{\mathbf u}\) \(\newcommand{\vvec}{\mathbf v}\) \(\newcommand{\wvec}{\mathbf w}\) \(\newcommand{\xvec}{\mathbf x}\) \(\newcommand{\yvec}{\mathbf y}\) \(\newcommand{\zvec}{\mathbf z}\) \(\newcommand{\rvec}{\mathbf r}\) \(\newcommand{\mvec}{\mathbf m}\) \(\newcommand{\zerovec}{\mathbf 0}\) \(\newcommand{\onevec}{\mathbf 1}\) \(\newcommand{\real}{\mathbb R}\) \(\newcommand{\twovec}[2]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\ctwovec}[2]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\threevec}[3]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\cthreevec}[3]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\fourvec}[4]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\cfourvec}[4]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\fivevec}[5]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \\ #5 \\ \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\cfivevec}[5]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \\ #5 \\ \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\mattwo}[4]{\left[\begin{array}{rr}#1 \amp #2 \\ #3 \amp #4 \\ \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\laspan}[1]{\text{Span}\{#1\}}\) \(\newcommand{\bcal}{\cal B}\) \(\newcommand{\ccal}{\cal C}\) \(\newcommand{\scal}{\cal S}\) \(\newcommand{\wcal}{\cal W}\) \(\newcommand{\ecal}{\cal E}\) \(\newcommand{\coords}[2]{\left\{#1\right\}_{#2}}\) \(\newcommand{\gray}[1]{\color{gray}{#1}}\) \(\newcommand{\lgray}[1]{\color{lightgray}{#1}}\) \(\newcommand{\rank}{\operatorname{rank}}\) \(\newcommand{\row}{\text{Row}}\) \(\newcommand{\col}{\text{Col}}\) \(\renewcommand{\row}{\text{Row}}\) \(\newcommand{\nul}{\text{Nul}}\) \(\newcommand{\var}{\text{Var}}\) \(\newcommand{\corr}{\text{corr}}\) \(\newcommand{\len}[1]{\left|#1\right|}\) \(\newcommand{\bbar}{\overline{\bvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\bhat}{\widehat{\bvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\bperp}{\bvec^\perp}\) \(\newcommand{\xhat}{\widehat{\xvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\vhat}{\widehat{\vvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\uhat}{\widehat{\uvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\what}{\widehat{\wvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\Sighat}{\widehat{\Sigma}}\) \(\newcommand{\lt}{<}\) \(\newcommand{\gt}{>}\) \(\newcommand{\amp}{&}\) \(\definecolor{fillinmathshade}{gray}{0.9}\)

    Interbrain Neural Synchrony

    Infants are active in their interactions with other people, engaging with them in synchronous and dynamic exchanges (De Jaegher & Di Paolo, 2007; Schilbach et al., 2013). Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\) displays two images, each showing a type of synchrony that takes place in the everyday interactions between infants and toddlers and their caregivers. The image on the left of Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\) shows an infant and caregiver engaged in dyadic synchrony–they are looking at each other’s faces, sharing eye gaze, emotions, behaviors, etc., The image on the right of Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\) shows a toddler and caregiver engaged in triadic synchrony–they are sharing interest in an object through their shared gaze, emotions, behavior, etc. Interactive coordination, like in these two types of synchrony, is often referred to as interpersonal synchrony, a “dynamic process by which hormonal, physiological, and behavioral cues are exchanged” and reciprocally adjusted between conversational partners (Feldman, 2012). In this way, a synchronous interaction between caregivers and infants and toddlers involves more than shared interest and eye gaze–sharing also takes place at the physiological (e.g., oxytocin, cardiac output) and neural level (Feldman, 2007; Markova & Nguyen, 2023). [1]

    Definition: Interpersonal synchrony

    A dynamic process by which hormonal, physiological, and behavioral cues are exchanged and reciprocally adjusted between conversational partners

    image one infant and caregiver looking at one another for Dyadic Synchrony. Child looking away while the caregiver looks at them for Triadic Synchrony.
    Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\):Forms of interpersonal synchrony in caregiver-child interactions. Dyadic synchrony and Triadic Synchrony.   [2

    The interpersonal synchrony between an infant and caregiver can be seen in their brains. ​​At the neural level, interbrain synchrony can be defined as a dyadic mechanism, wherein temporally coordinated patterns of brain activity between two interacting individuals supports aspects of their ongoing social interaction (Holroyd, 2022). Interpersonal synchronization of brain rhythms may play a substantive role for caregiver-child coordination, communication, and attachment formation (Atzil & Gendron, 2017). Caregiver and infant face-to-face communication that includes shared gaze and vocalizations elicits greater interbrain synchrony as compared with moments of similar caregiver and infant proximity that do not include facial or vocal communication. Based on existing evidence, short-term outcomes of interbrain synchrony include enhanced social connectedness, effective communication as well as interpersonal regulation (Feldman, 2007; Leong et al., 2017; Stephens et al., 2010). In the long term, interbrain synchronization has been linked to the development of social competence, secure attachment and bonding (Atzil & Gendron, 2017).  [3] [4] [5]

    Definition: Interbrain synchrony

    Temporally coordinated patterns of brain activity between two interacting individuals supports aspects of their ongoing social interaction

    Caregiver attempts to engage infant in play while both have spectroscopy caps on
    Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\): Dual EEG experiment studying inter-brain synchrony. [6]

    To explore the interpersonal synchronization of brain activity between infants and caregivers engaging in face-to-face turn-taking conversations, researchers have used functional-near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) hyperscanning (Nguyen, Zimmer, & Hoehl, 2023). In developmental research, hyperscanning comprises the simultaneous measurement of brain activity in caregiver and child using different neuroimaging methods such as fNIRS, electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG). [7]

    Definition: Magnetoencephalography (MEG)

    A brain monitoring technique that allows for ongoing measurements of brain function, even in young infants

    Definition: Hyperscanning

    In developmental research, hyperscanning comprises the simultaneous measurement of brain activity in a caregiver and a child, sometimes using different neuroimaging methods

    Definition: Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS)

    A brain monitoring technique that measures hemodynamic (blood flow) responses elicited from neuronal activation by shining near-infrared light into the brain

    Definition: Electroencephalography (EEG)

    A brain monitoring technique that measures neuronal electrical activity using electrodes placed around the scalp

    infant in caregivers lap. Only infant is wearing cap.
    Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\): Infant wearing a functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) cap. [8]

     

    The results from the (fNIRS) hyperscanning (Nguyen, Zimmer, & Hoehl, 2023) study are eye-opening! The more turn-taking caregivers and infants shared, the more they displayed interpersonal neural synchrony during conversations. Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\) displays this data. Looking at Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\), the y-axis shows the level of neural synchrony between caregiver and infant, the x-axis shows the time engaged in the interaction and the colors represent the frequency of turn-taking, green being the highest frequency and red being the lowest frequency. The relation between neural synchrony and turn-taking frequency was stronger in the initial phase of the interaction and decreased over time. You can see this if you follow the green line across the chart, from left to right. The green line is the highest line early on in the interaction, showing that this is when the relationship between neural synchrony and turn-taking is the strongest. As interactions went on, the relationship between turn-taking and neural synchrony decreased (shown by the green line dropping downward across the chart). [9]

    see figure caption
    Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\):The graph depicts the interaction effect between interval (x axis) and the number of turns (traces, divided in −1 s.d. = red, estimated mean = blue, +1 s.d. = green) on interpersonal neural synchrony (measured in WTC; y axis). A higher turn-taking frequency is associated with higher neural synchrony between mother–infant at the beginning of the proto-conversation. Neural synchrony in later intervals is not associated with the number of turns. The shaded area depicts the 95% confidence interval of the predicted values.
    [10]

    This relationship between neural synchrony and turn-taking primarily occurred in the medial prefrontal cortex (Nguyen, Zimmer, & Hoehl, 2023). Medial prefrontal regions have been implicated in caregiver–infant synchrony, especially in face-to-face exchanges (Piazza et al., 2020). This is consistent with the role of the medial prefrontal cortex in processing communicative signals (Amodio & Frith, 2006; Kampe et al., 2003; Schilbach et al., 2006). In addition, the number of turn-taking in general, but especially infants' turns, was related to a larger vocabulary at twenty-four months of age. [11]

    Definition: Medial prefrontal cortex 

    Located in the middle area of the prefrontal cortex and has essential roles in various processes, such as early social cognition

    caregiver blows bubble toward infant while both are wearing nuero feedback caps
    Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\):Measuring neural synchrony using EEG. [12]

    Next, the researchers (Nguyen, Zimmer, & Hoehl, 2023) examined the relationship between caregiver–infant turn-taking and infant brain maturity, measured by inter-hemispheric functional connectivity. Functional connectivity within brain networks can be detected from very early in brain development, even in newborns (Fransson et al., 2009; Gao et al., 2015; Schöpf, Kasprian, Brugger, Prayer, 2012; Thomason et al., 2019; Zhang, Shen, & Lin, 2019). Research points to a developmental progression whereby functional connectivity in some brain networks are already in place at birth, whereas functional connectivity in higher-order brain networks show more protracted development during infancy (Gao et al., 2015; van den Heuvel & Thomason, 2016). Results showed that more frequent infant and caregiver turn-taking was positively related to brain maturation. This means that when caregivers and infants engage in more turn-taking, infants show greater inter-hemispheric functional connectivity. These results support previous research finding that infants' individual differences in inter-hemispheric connectivity can be identified and related to the individual differences they experience, such as turn-taking frequency (Allievi et al., 2016; Kelsey, Farris, & Grossmann, 2021; Nguyen, Zimmer, & Hoehl, 2023). The results emphasize the role of interpersonal neural synchrony in early turn-taking as well as brain maturation as a potential outcome related to early caregiver–infant interactions. [13] [14]

     

    infant and mother being MEG scanned while looking at images of one another
    Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\): MEG hyperscanning of a mother and infant. [15]

    Taken together, interbrain research shows that caregiver–infant interactions and conversations involve a complex exchange where the two become synchronized, even at the neural level. To establish interbrain synchronization, caregivers should ensure that they tune-in to children’s cues and take the time to engage with infants and toddlers, irrespective of the activity, whether it's playing with materials, meal time or changing diapers.  Importantly, synchronization involves the caregiver and children–interactions should not be one-sided. Additionally, caregivers who are more sensitive in their interactions show more caregiver–infant neural synchrony (Endevelt-Shapira & Feldman, 2023). When caregivers are synchronized with children, it leads to greater interbrain synchronization and brain maturation, which supports developmental outcomes. [16]

    Attributions:



    5.5: Interbrain Neural Synchrony is shared under a not declared license and was authored, remixed, and/or curated by LibreTexts.

    • Was this article helpful?