At first Amy is surprised that their two-week-old daughter Karina turns her head when she hears Amy’s voice. But as Amy thinks about this behavior, they realize that since hearing develops before birth, Karina has been listening to their voice for many months now. Amy mentions this observation to a friend who is an audiologist, who tells them that hearing is one of a newborn’s most highly developed senses.
The development of the senses is a crucial achievement in the first two years of life. Healthy infants are born with the ability to detect visual, auditory, tactile, gustatory (taste), and olfactory (smell) sensations. As they grow and develop, they become more adept at decoding these stimuli, a skill that helps them make sense of the world around them. Sensory development relies on both sensation, the detection of sensory stimuli, and perception, the interpretation of that sensory information. In this section, you’ll explore sensory development in infants and toddlers.
References
Adolph, K. E., & Kretch, K. S. (2012). Infants on the edge. Developmental Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies, 36–55. Sage Publications Ltd.
André, V., Henry, S., Vuillemin, A., Beuchée, A., Sizun, J., Roué, J. M., Lemasson, A., Misery, L., Hausberger, M., & Durier, V. (2020). A novel, short and easy–to–perform method to evaluate newborns’ social olfactory preferences. Animal Cognition, 23, 843–850. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-020-01397-w
Bakalar, N. (2012). Sensory science: Partners in flavour. Nature, 486, S4–S5. https://doi.org/10.1038/486S4a
Bower, C., Reilly, B. K., Richerson, J., & Hecht, J. L. (2023). Hearing assessment in infants, children, and adolescents: Recommendations beyond neonatal screening. Pediatrics, 152(3), Article e2023063288. doi.org/10.1542/peds.2023-063288
Bayet, L., Quinn, P. C., Tanaka, J. W., Lee, K., Gentaz, É., Pascalis, O. (2015). Face gender Influences the looking preference for smiling expressions in 3.5-month-old human infants. PLoS One, 10(6), Article e0129812. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129812
Carozza, S., & Leong, V. (2021). The role of affectionate caregiver touch in early neurodevelopment and parent–infant interactional synchrony. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 14, Article 613378. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2020.613378
Choi, D., Dehaene–Lambertz, G., Peña, M., & Werker, J. F. (2021). Neural indicators of articulator-specific sensorimotor influences on infant speech perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(20), Article e2025043118. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2025043118
Clifford, A., Franklin, A., Davies, I. R., & Holmes, A. (2009). Electrophysiological markers of categorical perception of color in 7–month old infants. Brain and Cognition, 71(2), 165–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.05.002
Dahl, A., Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Uchiyama, I., Witherington, D. C., Ueno, M., Poutrain–Lejeune, L., & Barbu–Roth, M. (2013). The epigenesis of wariness of heights. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1361–1367. doi.org/10.1177/0956797613476047
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science, 208(4448), 1174–1176. doi.org/10.1126/science.7375928
DeCasper, A. J., & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(2), 133–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(86)90025-1
Della Longa, L., Filippetti, M. L., Dragovic, D., & Farroni, T. (2020). Synchrony of caresses: Does affective touch help infants to detect body–related visual–tactile synchrony? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2944. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02944
Dunn, R. L., & Lessen, R. (2017). The influence of human milk on flavor and food preferences. Current Nutrition Reports, 6, 134–140. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13668-017-0200-3
Edgar, E. V., Todd, J. T., & Bahrick, L. E. (2022). Intersensory matching of faces and voices in infancy predicts language outcomes in young children. Developmental Psychology, 58(8), 1413–1428. doi.org/10.1037/dev0001375
Fadlalmola, H.A., Elhusein, A.M., Abedelwahed, H.H., Mohmed, S.A., Alnassry, A. M.A., Mohammed, F.H., Eltaher, N.S., Ebrahim, E. E., Abdelsadig, M. A., Ahmed, S. O.M., Farg, S. J., Ahmed, A. S. M., Mohmmed, S. S., Ali, A. M., Banaga, A. E., Mohammed, I. H., El-Amin, E. I., Moafa, H. N., Mohamed, A. H., Balola, H. H. A., Idress, E. A., Abraham, N., Sagiron, E. I. A., Jarelnape, A. A., Alhajeri, S. T., Alazhary, A. A., Alhawsawi, A. O., & Idris, A.M. (2023). The effects of Yakson touch and gentle human touch on preterm infants: A systematic review and meta–analysis. African Journal of Reproductive Health, 27(7), 99–108. https://doi.org/10.29063/ajrh2023/v27i7.10
Forestell, C. A. (2017). Flavor perception and preference development in human infants. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism, 70(Suppl. 3), 17–25. doi.org/10.1159/000478759
Forestell, C. A. (2020). You are what your parents eat: Parental influences on early flavor preference development. Building Future Health and Well–Being of Thriving Toddlers and Young Children, 95, 78–87. doi.org/10.1159/000511516
Forestell, C. A. (2024). Does maternal diet influence future infant taste and odor preferences? A critical analysis. Annual Review of Nutrition, 44. doi.org/10.1146/annurev-nutr-121222-101404
Gabriel, A. S., & Mennella, J. A. (2023). Umami taste: Inborn and experiential effects on taste acceptance and satiation during infancy. In Umami: Taste for Health (pp. 127–145). Cham: Springer International Publishing.
Garcia–Rodriguez, M. T., Bujan–Bravo, S., Seijo–Bestilleiro, R., & Gonzalez–Martin, C. (2021). Pain assessment and management in the newborn: A systematized review. World Journal of Clinical Cases, 9(21), 5921–5931. https://doi.org/10.12998/wjcc.v9.i21.5921
Gibson, E.J., & Walk, R.D. (1960). Visual cliff. Scientific American.
Hepper, P. G. (1988). Adaptive fetal learning: Prenatal exposure to garlic affects personal preferences. Animal Behaviour, 36, 935–936. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80177-5
Ichikawa, H., Tsuruhara, A., Kanazawa, S., & Yamaguchi, M. K. (2013). Two–to three–month–old infants prefer moving face patterns to moving top–heavy patterns. Japanese Psychological Research, 55(3), 254–263. doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00540.x
Johnson, S. P. (2011). Development of visual perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(5), 515–528. doi.org/10.1002/wcs.128
Johnson, S. P., & Hannon, E. E. (2015). Perceptual development. In L. S. Liben, U. Müller, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes (7th ed., pp. 63–112). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy203
Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Charles–Luce, J. (1994). Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(5), 630–645. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1030
Kavšek, M., Yonas, A., & Granrud, C. E. (2012). Infants’ sensitivity to pictorial depth cues: A review and meta–analysis of looking studies. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(1), 109–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.08.003
Kellman, P. J., & Arterberry, M. E. (2006). Infant visual perception. In D. Kuhn, R. S. Siegler, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Cognition, Perception, and Language (6th ed., pp. 109–160). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Kisilevsky, B. S. (2016). Fetal auditory processing: Implications for language development? Fetal development: Research on Brain and Behavior, Environmental Influences, and Emerging Technologies, 133–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-22023-9_8
Kragness, H. E., Johnson, E. K., & Cirelli, L. K. (2022). The song, not the singer: Infants prefer to listen to familiar songs, regardless of singer identity. Developmental Science, 25(1), Article e13149. doi.org/10.1111/desc.13149
Lewkowicz, D. J. (1999). The development of temporal and spatial intermodal perception. In Advances in Psychology (Vol. 129, pp. 395–420). North–Holland. doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.281
Lipchock, S. V., Reed, D. R., & Mennella, J. A. (2011). The gustatory and olfactory systems during infancy: implications for development of feeding behaviors in the high-risk neonate. Clinics in Perinatology, 38(4), 627–641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2011.08.008
Loos, H. M., Reger, D., & Schaal, B. (2019). The odour of human milk: Its chemical variability and detection by newborns. Physiology & Behavior, 199, 88–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.11.008
May, L., Gervain, J., Carreiras, M., & Werker, J. F. (2018). The specificity of the neural response to speech at birth. Developmental Science, 21(3), Article e12564. doi.org/10.1111/desc.12564
Mehrpisheh, S., Doorandish, Z., Farhadi, R., Ahmadi, M., Moafi, M., & Elyasi, F. (2022). The Effectiveness of Kangaroo Mother Care (KMC) on attachment of mothers with premature infants. European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology: X, 15, Article 100149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurox.2022.100149
Mennella, J. A. (2014). Ontogeny of taste preferences: basic biology and implications for health. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 99(3), 704S–711S. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.113.067694
Patel, J. K., Rouster, A. S. (2023). Infant Nutrition Requirements and Options. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK560758/
Porter, R. H., Makin, J. W., Davis, L. B., & Christensen, K. M. (1992). Breast–fed infants respond to olfactory cues from their own mother and unfamiliar lactating females. Infant Behavior and Development, 15(1), 85–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(92)90008-T
Seidl, A. H., Indarjit, M., & Borovsky, A. (2023). Touch to learn: Multisensory input supports word learning and processing. Developmental Science, Article e13419. doi.org/10.1111/desc.13419
Skelton, A. E., & Franklin, A. (2020). Infants look longer at colours that adults like when colours are highly saturated. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(1), 78–85. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01688-5
Streri, A., & de Hevia, M. D. (2023). How do human newborns come to understand the multimodal environment?. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30(4), 1171–1186. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-023-02260-y
Sullivan, R., Perry, R., Sloan, A., Kleinhaus, K., Burtchen, N. (2011). Infant bonding and attachment to the caregiver: insights from basic and clinical science. Clinics in Perinatology, 38(4), 643–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2011.08.011
Wang, Z. W., Hua, J., & Xu, Y. H. (2015). The relationship between gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus and its temperament 3 months after birth. Behavioural Neurology, 2015(1), Article 371906. doi.org/10.1155/2015/371906
Wiggins, S., & Keevallik, L. (2021). Parental lip-smacks during infant mealtimes: Multimodal features and social functions. Interactional Linguistics, 1(2), 241–272. https://doi.org/10.1075/il.21006.wig
Wilcox, T., Stubbs, J. A., Wheeler, L., & Alexander, G. M. (2013). Infants' scanning of dynamic faces during the first year. Infant Behavior & Development, 36(4), 513–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.05.001
Yildirim, F., Duman, N. B., Şahin, E., & Vural, G. (2023). The effect of kangaroo care on paternal attachment: A randomized controlled study. Advances in Neonatal Care, 23(6), 596–601. doi.org/10.1097/anc.0000000000001100