Six-month-old Ibrahim has just woken from his nap, and his father Ahmed hears him babbling “da da da da” over the intercom. At first Ahmed is excited because it sounds like the baby is calling him. However, as he listens, he begins to wonder whether this is just another example of the babbling he first heard last week, when Ibrahim was saying “ba ba ba.” As he lifts his son out of his crib, Ahmed expresses his delight in seeing Ibrahim and begins to have a conversation with him in a higher-than-normal pitch. He narrates everything he is doing while changing his son’s diaper, and Ibrahim smiles as he talks and resumes babbling when he pauses. Father and son are both fully enjoying their conversation. Babbling and turn taking are just a couple of the important milestones in the development of language that Ibrahim will be achieving during his first two years of life.
Language development is a complex process influenced by both nature and nurture. As infants progress through their first year of life, they develop the components of language at a remarkable pace, laying the foundation for building more complex language abilities in the years to come.
References
Albareda–Castellot, B, Pons, F, & Sebastián–Gallés, N. (2011). The acquisition of phonetic categories in bilingual infants: New data from an anticipatory eye movement paradigm. Developmental Science, 14, 395–401. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000348
Ashtari, A., Samadi, S. A., Yadegari, F., & Ghaedamini Harooni, G. (2020). The relationship between Iranian maternal verbal responsiveness styles and child's communication acts with expressive and receptive vocabulary in 13–18 months old typically developing children. Early Child Development and Care, 190(15), 2392–2401. doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2019.1573227
Baker, C. (2011). Foundations of bilingual education and bilingualism. McNaughton & Gunn.
Baldwin, D. A. (2014). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. Joint attention. (pp. 131–158). Psychology Press.
Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development: Six theories of child development (Vol. 6, pp. 1–60) Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
Barnes E., & Puccioni J. (2017). Shared book reading and preschool children’s academic achievement: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study—birth cohort. Infant and Child Development, 26(6), Article e2035. doi.org/10.1002/icd.2035
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 3253–3258. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113380109
Bruner, J. (1983). Play, thought, and language. Peabody Journal of Education, 60(3), 60–69.
Brushe, M. E., Haag, D. G., Melhuish, E. C., Reilly, S., & Gregory, T. (2024). Screen time and parent-child talk when children are aged 12 to 36 months. JAMA Pediatrics, 178(4), 369–375. jamanetwork.com/journals/jam...rticle/2815514
Byers–Heinlein, K., & Lew-Williams, C. (2013). Bilingualism in the early years: What the science says. LEARNing landscapes, 7(1), 95. https://doi.org/10.36510/learnland.v7i1.632
Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and mind. New York: Harquort, Brace and World.
Cleave, P. L., Becker, S. D., Curran, M. K., Van Horne, A. J., & Fey, M. E. (2015). The efficacy of recasts in language intervention: A systematic review and meta–analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 24(2), 237–255. doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0105
Clerkin, E. M., & Smith, L. B. (2022). Real-world statistics at two timescales and a mechanism for infant learning of object names. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(18), Article e2123239119. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2123239119
Cooper, R.P., Abrahan, J., Berman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20(4), 477–488. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90037-0
Crais, E., Douglas, D. D., & Campbell, C. C. (2004). The intersection of the development of gestures and intentionality. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(3), 678–694. doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/052)
Crowe, K., & McLeod, S. (2020). Children's English consonant acquisition in the United States: A review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(4), 2155–2169. doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00168
De Meulder, M. (2019). " So, why do you sign?" Deaf and hearing new signers, their motivation, and revitalisation policies for sign languages. Applied Linguistics Review, 10(4). https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2017-0100
Farran, L. K., Lee, C.–C., Yoo, H., & Oller, D. K. (2016). Cross-cultural register differences in infant-directed speech: An initial study. PLOS ONE, 11(3), Article e0151518. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151518
Festman, J., Poarch, G. J., & Dewaele, J. M. (2017). Raising multilingual children (Vol. 23). Multilingual Matters.
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14. doi.org/10.1037/a0027238
Humphreys, K.L., King, L.S., Guyon-Harris, K.L., Sheridan, M.A., McLaughlin, K.A., Radulescu, A., Nelson, C.A., Fox, N.A., & Zeanah, C.H. (2022). Foster care leads to sustained cognitive gains following severe early deprivation. PNAS, 119(38), Article e2119318119. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2119318119
Jackson, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., & Boyes, M. E. (2019). Fast mapping short and long words: Examining the influence of phonological short-term memory and receptive vocabulary in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Communication Disorders, 79, 11–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.02.001
Ji, C., Mudiyanselage, T. B., Gao, Y., & Pan, Y. (2021). A review of infant cry analysis and classification. EURASIP Journal on Audio, Speech, and Music Processing, 2021(1), Article 8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13636-021-00197-5
Kirk, E., Howlett, N., Pine, K. J., & Fletcher, B. C. (2013). To sign or not to sign? The impact of encouraging infants to gesture on infant language and maternal mind–mindedness. Society for Research in Child Development, 84(2), 574–90. doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01874.x
Kucker, S. C., Perry, L. K., & Barr, R. (2024). Variability and patterns in children's media use and links with language development. Acta Paediatrica, 113(5), 1032–1039. doi.org/10.1111/apa.17100
Laing, C., & Bergelson, E. (2020). From babble to words: Infants’ early productions match words and objects in their environment. Cognitive psychology, 122, Article 101308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2020.101308
Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3(3), 299–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(88)90014-7
Lillo–Martin, D., & Henner, J. (2021). Acquisition of sign languages. Annual Review of Linguistics, 7, 395–419. doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ling...-043020-092357
Lytle, S. R., Garcia–Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2018). Two are better than one: Infant language learning from video improves in the presence of peers. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(40), 9859–9866. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1611621115
Madigan, S., Prime, H., Graham, S. A., Rodrigues, M., Anderson, N., Khoury, J., & Jenkins, J. M. (2019). Parenting behavior and child language: A meta–analysis. Pediatrics, 144(4), Article e20183556. doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-3556
Maneva, B., & Genesee, F. (2002). Bilingual babbling: Evidence for language differentiation in dual language acquisition. Boston University Conference on Language Development 26, Proceedings. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Many Babies Consortium. (2020). Quantifying sources of variability in infancy research using the infant-directed speech preference. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(1), 24–52. doi.org/10.1177/2515245919900809
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5
Morgan, L., & Wren, Y. E. (2018). A systematic review of the literature on early vocalizations and babbling patterns in young children. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 40(1), 3–14. doi.org/10.1177/1525740118760215
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychological Review, 81(4), 267. doi.org/10.1037/h0036592
Nelson, C. A., Furtado, E. A., Fox, N. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2009). The deprived human brain: Developmental deficits among institutionalized Romanian children—and later improvements—strengthen the case for individualized care. American Scientist, 97(3), 222–229. doi.org/10.1511/2009.78.222
Novack, M. A., Chan, D., & Waxman, S. (2022). I see what you are saying: Hearing infants’ visual attention and social engagement in response to spoken and sign language. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, Article 896049. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.896049
Ortiz–Mantilla, S., Realpe–Bonilla, T., & Benasich, A. A. (2019). Early interactive acoustic experience with non-speech generalizes to speech and confers a syllabic processing advantage at 9 months. Cerebral Cortex, 29(4), 1789–1801. doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz001
Peixoto, C., Cadima, J., & Leal, T. (2022). Mothers’ educational level and literacy beliefs: Associations with home literacy experiences. International Journal of Early Years Education, 30(2), 167–183. doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2022.2025580
Plummer, A. R., & Beckman, M. E. (2015). Framing a socio–indexical basis for the emergence and cultural transmission of phonological systems. Journal of Phonetics, 53, 66–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.09.004
Preston, J. L., Hull, M., & Edwards, M. L. (2013). Preschool speech error patterns predict articulation and phonological awareness outcomes in children with histories of speech sound disorders. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 22(2), 173–184. doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/12-0022)
Shahid, M. A., Syed, A. F., Razi, S. K. A., Sajid, S., & Hussain, I. (2021). Phonological process in toddlers’’ single–word production: An explorative study of alveolar sounds in English. Journal of English Language Teaching and Applied Linguistics, 3(3), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.32996/jeltal.2021.3.3.7
Shi, J., Gu, Y., & Vigliocco, G. (2022). Prosodic modulations in child–directed language and their impact on word learning. Developmental Science, 26(4), Article 13357. doi.org/10.1111/desc.13357
Skinner B.F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts.
Sung, J., & Hsu, H. (2009). Korean mothers’ attention regulation and referential speech: Associations with language and play in 1–year–olds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(5), 430–439. doi.org/10.1177/0165025409338443
Syafitri, B. A. (2021, April). From perception to production: An analysis of first language acquisition in 2.5 Years old toddler speech. English Language and Literature International Conference (ELLiC) Proceedings (Vol. 4, pp. 307–312).
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Troseth, G. L., Strouse, G. A., Verdine, B. N., & Saylor, M. M. (2018). Let’s chat: On–screen social responsiveness is not sufficient to support toddlers’ word learning from video. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2195. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02195
Vannasing, P., Florea, O., González-Frankenberger, B., Tremblay, J., Paquette, N., Safi, D., Wallois, F., Lepore, F., Béland, R., Lassonde, M., & Gallagher, A. (2016). Distinct hemispheric specializations for native and non-native languages in one-day-old newborns identified by fNIRS. Neuropsychologia, 84, 63–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsych...ia.2016.01.038
Vallotton, C. D., Mastergeorge, A., Foster, T., Decker, K. B., & Ayoub, C. (2017). Parenting supports for early vocabulary development: Specific effects of sensitivity and stimulation through infancy. Infancy, 22(1), 78–107. doi.org/10.1111/infa.12147
Vehkavuori, S. M., Kämäräinen, M., & Stolt, S. (2021). Early receptive and expressive lexicons and language and pre-literacy skills at 5; 0 years–A longitudinal study. Early Human Development, 156, Article 105345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2021.105345
Vermillet, A. Q., Tølbøll, K., Litsis Mizan, S., C Skewes, J., & Parsons, C. E. (2022). Crying in the first 12 months of life: A systematic review and meta‐analysis of cross‐country parent–reported data and modeling of the “cry curve”. Child Development, 93(4), 1201–1222. doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13760
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(1), 49–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80022-3
Werker, J. F., Pegg, J. E., & McLeod, P. J. (1994). A cross-language investigation of infant preference for infant–directed communication. Infant Behavior and Development, 17(3), 323–333. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(94)90012-4
Werwach, A., Mürbe, D., Schaadt, G., & Männel, C. (2021). Infants’ vocalizations at 6 months predict their productive vocabulary at one year. Infant Behavior and Development, 64, 101588. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101588
Windsor, J., Glaze, L. E., & Koga, S. F. (2007). Language acquisition with limited input: Romanian institution and foster care. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1365–1381. doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/095)
Yee, S. (2020). Is noun bias universal? Evidence from Chinese and Korean compared with French and English. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences: Illinois Working Papers. http://hdl.handle.net/2142/109008