Renae enjoys their work as a preschool teacher. Even on the rough days with tantrums, spilled paint, and bathroom accidents, Renae knows their day-to-day work allows them to witness something extraordinary. During early childhood, the students’ brains will grow, facilitating an expansion of cognitive abilities. By the time these students transition to middle childhood and early elementary school, they’ll be solving increasingly complex cognitive tasks, including jigsaw puzzles, math calculations, and negotiations to acquire a family pet.
In this section, you’ll learn about the cognitive milestones expected in early childhood through the lens of various cognitive theories that have had a significant influence on how psychologists view this growth and development. Scientists, scholars, and practitioners use these theoretical lenses as a foundation to understand how children acquire natural skills like language, as well as those that are more culturally nuanced, like learning to be independent versus relying on the guidance of others.
Information Processing Theory
The information processing theory views cognitive development as a continuous loop of information processing and storage, as you learned in 3.4 Cognition and Memory in Infants and Toddlers. Individuals initially attend to incoming information, which is then processed and organized so that it can be efficiently stored. Memories are continually compared and evaluated against each another, allowing for systematic retrieval of information. More advanced cognitive skills develop when we store new information, compare it with existing memories, and generate appropriate responses.
Information processing includes the interrelated processes of executive function, attention, and memory. These cognitive processes play important roles in helping children learn, problem-solve, and adapt to their environment. Recall that executive function includes skills like planning, organizing, decision-making, and impulse control. These also necessitate the use of attention, which is the ability to focus on specific stimuli while ignoring distractions. That is, individuals need to attend to multiple bits of information when they plan, organize, and make decisions.
People also need to diminish distractions, a process called inhibitory control, which allows regulation of impulses and focus. These components of information processing begin developing in early childhood when infants have limited attention spans. With time, children become better at sustaining attention as well as shifting efficiently between tasks. In addition, executive function includes actions related to memory.
Memory Development
To process information, our memory systems encode, store, and eventually retrieve the information. In this way, information processing theory explains how memory systems help us retain knowledge, recall past experiences, and build a foundation for cognitive advancement and learning.
Another major milestone of memory development in early childhood is the ability to hold an autobiographical memory. Autobiographical memories are long-term memories of individual life experiences and events. Our earliest memories often begin to develop in early childhood and continue to improve through adolescence (Harley & Reese, 1999; Bauer & Larkina, 2017). Development of this type of memory can help children recall past experiences, develop self-knowledge, and shape their growing sense of identity over time (Ross et al., 2020; Habermas &. Reese, 2015).
Autobiographical memory can be improved and scaffolded by caregivers. When caregivers engage in past-talk conversations, such as helping a child remember a recent family gathering or event, children show improved autobiographical memory, socioemotional skills, and self-regulation (Levya et al., 2019; Wu & Jobson, 2019). For example, a parent may help a four-year-old better recall a grandparent who lives far away by asking, “Do you remember when Grandpa visited and we all went to the pumpkin patch? The pumpkins were so bright and we rode in a truck with hay bales!” Prompts like this can help children improve their autobiographical memory and better remember grandpa before his next visit.
The Continuous Nature of Cognitive Development
The information processing theory builds on foundational theories, including Piaget's cognitive stage theory. It provides a model that acknowledges the maturational changes outlined by Piaget, while also incorporating the modern, expanded view that environmental circumstances and executive functions matter. For example, both theories agree that a five-year-old child would not be expected to understand advanced algebra. However, they have different views regarding a child's potential to later learn the complexities of math.
Piaget theorized that development consisted of step-like, qualitative changes. In information processing theory, the quantity of information is a significant factor in cognitive growth. Although substantial evidence confirms Piaget’s theory that the order of stages is fixed, children’s progression through them may either be accelerated or delayed (Winstanley, 2023). The social environment, which includes schools and caregivers, also plays an important role.
The continuous nature of development, including both qualitative and quantitative changes, is exemplified in nearly everything we learn. For example, our understanding of math is a progression from simply counting to performing elementary arithmetic, to tackling more advanced concepts. However, like upgrading a device to a more powerful processor, maturation allows us to think differently about math as we develop, demonstrating the complementary nature of Piaget’s stages and information processing. Psychologists can theorize that thinking matures in a similar way during the development of our understanding of oral language, music, biology, and other subject matters.
We also update our processing skills by continually observing and imitating the behavior of others. The complementary nature of information processing and learning theories is evident in the way we acquire reading skills. To decode words, we rely on our accumulated knowledge of sounds and meanings. In addition, we are constantly being exposed to new vocabulary, assorted word meanings, and varied sentence structures. Thus, we experience a continual interplay between storage and processing of words, which facilitates our retrieval of previously acquired semantic memories. This cognitive feedback loop allows us to refine and expand our vocabulary, leading to improved reading and comprehension skills. We also grow increasingly proficient at expressing desires and understanding the world, a process that becomes naturally self-reinforcing.
References
Airenti, G. (2015). Theory of mind: A new perspective on the puzzle of belief ascription. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1184. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01184
Badakar, C. M., Thakkar, P. J., Hugar, S. M., Kukreja, P., Assudani, H. G., & Gokhale, N. (2017). Evaluation of the relevance of Piaget’s cognitive principles among parented and orphan children in Belagavi City, Karnataka, India: A comparative study. International Journal of Clinical Pediatric Dentistry, 10(4), 346–350. https://doi.org/10.5005/jp-journals-10005-1463
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8
Bauer, P. J., & Larkina, M. (2017). Predictors of age-related and individual variability in autobiographical memory in childhood. Memory, 27(1), 63–78. doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1381267
Coppens, A. D., & Rogoff, B. (2022). Cultural variation in the early development of initiative in children’s prosocial helping. Social Development, 31(3), 656–678. doi.org/10.1111/sode.12566
Coppens, A. D., Silva, K. G., Ruvalcaba, O., Alcalá, L., López, A., & Rogoff, B. (2014). Learning by observing and pitching in: Benefits and processes of expanding repertoires. Human Development, 57(2–3), 150–161. doi.org/10.1159/000356770
Dailey, S., & Bergelson, E. (2021). Language input to infants of different socioeconomic statuses: A quantitative meta-analysis. Developmental Science, 25(3), Article e13192. doi.org/10.1111/desc.13192
DeVries, R. (1997). Piaget's social theory. Educational Researcher, 26(2). doi.org/10.3102/0013189X026002004
Habermas, T., & Reese, E. (2015). Getting a life takes time: The development of the life story in adolescence, its precursors and consequences. Human Development, 58(3), 172–201. doi.org/10.1159/000437245
Harley, K., & Reese, E. (1999). Origins of autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1338–1348. doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.35.5.1338
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
Huang, Y. (2021). Comparison and contrast of Piaget and Vygotsky’s theories. In J. Wang, B. Achour, & C. Y. Huang (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Humanities and Social Science Research (ICHSSR 2021): Vol. 554 (pp. 28–32). Atlantis Press. https://doi.org/10.2991/assehr.k.210519.007
Hutton, J. S., Dudley, J., Horowitz-Kraus, T., DeWitt, T., & Holland, S. K. (2020). Associations between home literacy environment, brain white matter integrity and cognitive abilities in preschool-age children. Acta Paediatrica, 109(7), 1376–1386. doi.org/10.1111/apa.15124
Kirolos, A., Goyheneix, M., Eliasz, M. E., Chisala, M., Lissauer, S., Gladstone, M., & Kerack, M. (2022). Neurodevelopmental, cognitive, behavioural and mental health impairments following childhood malnutrition: A systematic review. BMJ Global Health, 7(7), Article e009330. dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmjgh-2022-009330
Kraatz, E., Nagpal, M., Lin, T-J., Hsieh, M-Y., Ha, S. Y., Kim, S., & Shin, S. (2019). Teacher scaffolding of social and intellectual collaboration in small groups: A comparative case study. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.587058
Leyva, D., Reese, E., Laible, D., Schaughency, E., Das, S., & Clifford, A. (2019). Measuring parents’ elaborative reminiscing: Differential links of parents’ elaboration to children’s autobiographical memory and socioemotional skills. Journal of Cognition and Development, 21(1), 23–45. doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1668395
Mitra, S. (2003). Minimally invasive education: A progress report on the “hole-in-the-wall” experiments. The British Journal of Educational Technology, 34(3), 367–371. doi.org/10.1111/1467-8535.00333
Mitra, S., & Dangwal, R. (2017). Acquisition of computer literacy skills through self-organizing systems of learning among children in Bhutan and India. Prospects, 47, 275–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11125-017-9409-6
Mitra, S., & Dangwal, R. (2022). Evolution of the “hole-in-the-wall”: A status review. Prospects, 52, 209–222. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11125-021-09552-y
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The child’s conception of space. W. W. Norton & Company.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512
Rogoff, B., Correa-Chávez, M., & Silva, K. G. (2011). Cultural variation in children’s attention and learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher, R. W. Pew, L. M. Hough, & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 154–163). Worth Publishers. psycnet.apa.org/record/2011-19926-020
Ross, J., Hutchison, J., & Cunningham, S. J. (2020). The me in memory: The role of the self in autobiographical memory development. Child Development, 91(2), e299–e314. doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13211
Sheridan, M. A., Mukerji, C. E., Wade, M., Humphreys, K. L., Garrisi, K., Goel, S., Patel, K., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., Nelson, C. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2022). Early deprivation alters structural brain development from middle childhood to adolescence. Science Advances, 8(40). doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abn4316
Tompkins, V. (2022). Relations between the home literacy environment and young children’s theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 62, Article 101179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101179
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128 https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
Winstanley, M. A. (2023). Stages in theory and experiment. Fuzzy-structuralism and Piagetian stages. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 57(1), 151–173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-022-09702-7
Wu, Y., & Jobson, L. (2019). Maternal reminiscing and child autobiographical memory elaboration: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 55(12), 2505–2521. doi.org/10.1037/dev0000821
Zhao, C., Shang, S., Compton, A. M., Fu, G., & Sai, L. (2021). A longitudinal study of the relations between theory of mind, executive function, and lying in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.766891
Zubler, J. M., Wiggins, L. D., Macias, M. M., Whitaker, T. M., Shaw, J. S., Squires, J. K., Pajek, J. A., Wolf, R. B., Slaughter, K. S., Broughton, A. S., Gerndt, K. L., Mlodoch, B. J. & Lipkin, P. H. (2022). Evidence-informed milestones for developmental surveillance tools. Pediatrics, 149(3), Article e2021052138. doi.org/10.1542/peds.2021-052138