Understand Kinsey's continuum of sexual orientation and the range of sexual orientation labels used now
Explain the evidence for at least two nonsocial causes of sexual orientation
Overview
This section discusses sexual orientation, a person's emotional and sexual attraction to other individuals of a particular sex or gender. Kinsey's continuum of sexual orientation (a range from exclusively heterosexual through equally bisexual to exclusively homosexual) is introduced, as well as some additional terms that are in contemporary use (polysexual, pansexual, and asexual). The development and origins of sexual orientation are addressed, including research exploring genetics, prenatal hormone exposure, the fraternal-birth-order effect, and childhood development. Regardless of the root cause(s) for a person's sexual orientation (and/or gender identity), it is not a conscious choice and cannot be easily changed.
Sexual Orientation
A person's sexual orientation is their emotional and sexual attraction to a particular sex or gender, including a continuing pattern of romantic or sexual attraction (or a combination of these) to persons of a given sex or gender. According to the American Psychological Association (APA) (2016), sexual orientation also refers to a person's sense of identity based on those attractions, related behaviors, and membership in a community of others who share those attractions. Although a person’s intimate behavior may have sexual fluidity- changing due to circumstances (Diamond, 2009)- sexual orientations are relatively stable over one’s lifespan, and are influenced by genetics (Frankowski, 2004). Sexual orientation is distinct and independent from both biological sex and gender, as discussed in Section 13.4.
While some argue that sexual attraction is primarily driven by reproduction (e.g., Geary, 1998), empirical studies point to pleasure as the primary force behind our sex drive. For example, in a survey of college students who were asked, “Why do people have sex?” respondents gave more than 230 unique responses, most of which were related to pleasure rather than reproduction (Meston & Buss, 2007). Here’s a thought-experiment to further demonstrate how reproduction has relatively little to do with driving sexual attraction: Add the number of times you’ve had and/or hope to have sex during your lifetime. With this number in mind, consider how many times the goal was (or will be) for reproduction versus how many it was (or will be) for pleasure. Which number is greater?
A Continuum of Sexual Orientation
Instead of thinking of sexual orientation as being two dichotomous categories- homosexual (attracted to the same sex, Figure \(\PageIndex{1}\)) and heterosexual (attracted to the opposite sex)- sexuality researcher Alfred Kinsey and colleagues argued that it is a continuum (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948). They measured sexual orientation using a seven-point scale called the Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating Scale (Figure \(\PageIndex{2}\)), in which zero is exclusively heterosexual, three is bisexual (with equal attractions to the same and opposite sexes), and six is exclusively homosexual. Research done over several decades has supported this idea that sexual orientation ranges along a continuum, from exclusive attraction to the opposite sex/gender to exclusive attraction to the same sex/gender (Carroll, 2016).
Figure \(\PageIndex{2}\): The Kinsey scale, a tool which enables an individual to select a response on a scale between heterosexual and homosexual. 0= exclusively heterosexual, 1= predominantly heterosexual, only incidentally homosexual, 2= predominantly heterosexual, more than incidentally homosexual, 3= equally heterosexual and homosexual, 4= predominantly homosexual, more than incidentally heterosexual, 5= predominantly homosexual, only incidentally heterosexual, and 6= exclusively homosexual.
The commonly stated notion that “10% of people are homosexual” originates (erroneously) from Kinsey’s 1948 study, which found that 10% of men reported being exclusively homosexual for at least three years during adulthood. In addition to the likelihood that Kinsey’s sample overestimated the actual occurrence of homosexuality in the population, only 4% of the male respondents in his study reported being homosexual their entire lifetime. This figure is much closer to the 3.5% prevalence of non-heterosexual (gay/lesbian/bisexual) orientation found in more recent and representative studies of US and other Western populations (Bailey et al., 2016). However, it is important to note that how a respondent is asked to report their sexual orientation produces very different results. Researchers using the Kinsey Scale have found 18% to 39% of Europeans and Americans identifying as somewhere between heterosexual and homosexual (Lucas et al., 2017; YouGov.com, 2015). These percentages drop dramatically to only 0.5% to 1.9% non-heterosexual when researchers force individuals to respond using only two categories (Copen, Chandra, & Febo-Vazquez, 2016; Gates, 2011).
Although the percentage of US adults identifying as LGBT (lesbian/gay/bisexual/transgender) remained relatively stable through the early 2010s (and still remains stable in older-aged cohorts), there is evidence that younger people are more likely to identify as LGBT than previous generations, which is in turn increasing the overall prevalence in the population. Gates (2017, page 1221) reports that “the percentage of older age cohorts identifying as LGBT has remained stable or declined despite large increases among Millennials, who are now three times more likely than Baby Boomers to identify as LGBT (7.3% vs 2.4%).” (Millennials are people born during the years 1981 through 1996 and Baby Boomers are people born during the years 1946 through 1964 (Dimock, 2019).)
Indeed, even beyond a continuum from heterosexual to homosexual, sexual orientation is as diverse as gender identity. Some examples of sexual orientation include heterosexuality (attraction to the opposite sex/gender), homosexuality (attraction to the same sex/gender, also referred to as same-sex attraction- some people find the term homosexuality offensive since it was previously classified as a mental illness), bisexuality (attraction to two sexes/genders), polysexuality (attraction to multiple sexes/genders), pansexuality (attraction to all sexes/genders), and asexuality (no sexual attraction to any sex/gender).
Development and Origins of Sexual Orientation
According to current scientific understanding, individuals are usually aware of their sexual orientation between middle childhood and early adolescence. However, this is not always the case, and some do not become aware of their sexual orientation until much later in life. It is not necessary to participate in sexual activity to be aware of these emotional, romantic, and physical attractions; people can be celibate (not participating in any type of sexual activity) and still recognize their sexual orientation. Some researchers argue that sexual orientation is not static and inborn, but is instead fluid and changeable throughout the lifespan. Regardless of when sexual orientation is recognized or how it is expressed, it is clearly not a conscious "choice" for either heterosexual or non-heterosexual individuals.
There is no scientific consensus regarding the exact reasons why an individual holds a particular sexual orientation. Research has examined possible biological, developmental, social, and cultural influences on sexual orientation, but there is no conclusive evidence that links sexual orientation to one specific factor (APA, 2016). In an extensive review of evidence for the causal mechanisms of human sexual orientation, Bailey et al. (2016, page 46) state that "no causal theory of sexual orientation has yet gained widespread support", further pointing out that the most probable scientific hypotheses are difficult to test. The most commonly suggested social causes of homosexuality, "sexual recruitment by homosexual adults, patterns of disordered parenting, or the influence of homosexual parents", generally have only weak support and are compromised by many confounding factors (Bailey et al., 2016, page 46). They conclude that "there is considerably more evidence supporting nonsocial causes of sexual orientation than social causes". Some of the prominent nonsocial causes that have been examined include genetics, prenatal hormone exposure, the fraternal-birth-order effect, and behavior differences during childhood development.
Genetics
One method of measuring the genetic roots of sexual orientation is concordance rates, which is the probability that a pair of individuals has the same sexual orientation. If both twins have the same sexual orientation, they are "concordant" for this trait. Concordance rates are calculated and compared between people who share the same genetics (monozygotic twins, 99%), some of the same genetics (dizygotic twins, 50%, and siblings, 50%), and non-related people (randomly selected from the population). Researchers find that concordance for sexual orientation is highest for monozygotic twins; and concordance rates for dizygotic twins, siblings, and randomly-selected pairs are not significantly different (Bailey et al. 2016; Kendler et al., 2000). Since concordance rates are most similar for monozygotic twins, this indicates that "nature" (genetics) influences the expression of sexual orientation. However, monozygotic twins do not always have the same sexual orientation, so "nurture" (the environment and individual experiences) also plays a role in determining sexual orientation. Nonetheless, because sexual orientation is a hotly debated issue, an appreciation of the genetic aspects of attraction can be an important piece of this dialogue.
Prenatal Hormone Exposure
Excess or deficient exposure to hormones during prenatal development has also been theorized as an explanation for non-heterosexual orientation. One-third of females exposed to abnormal amounts of prenatal androgens due to congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH, see the section on differences of sexual development) identify as bisexual or lesbian (Cohen-Bendahan, van de Beek, & Berenbaum, 2005). In contrast, too little exposure to prenatal androgens may affect male sexual orientation (Carlson, 2011).
Fraternal-Birth-Order Effect
The "fraternal-birth-order effect" is the well-documented (and cross-culturally robust) finding that homosexual males tend to have more older brothers than heterosexual males (Bailey et al., 2016). Interestingly, this effect is particular to right-handed homosexual males (Blanchard et al., 2006; Bogaert, 2007). This effect is also specific to biological brothers (Figure \(\PageIndex{3}\)), meaning that the number of sons that have been gestated by the man's mother is the important factor, whether or not the man was raised with those brothers. "The effect is almost certainly causal, with each additional older brother causing an increase in the chances of a man’s being homosexual. ... Assuming that a man without any older brothers has a 2% chance of being homosexual, a man with one older brother has a 2.6% chance; with two, three, and four older brothers, the chances are 3.5%, 4.6%, and 6.0%, respectively" (Bailey et al., 2016, page 79). One proposed mechanism for this is that the mother creates antibodies against proteins on her developing son's Y chromosome (a possibility which increases with each pregnancy of a son), and that those antibodies may prevent the normal functioning of subsequent sons' Y chromosomes (Bailey et al., 2016; O'Hanlan et al., 2018).
Figure \(\PageIndex{3}\): Four of the Marx Brothers in “I’ll Say She Is!”—caricatures by John Decker (1924). Every biological older brother increases the likelihood of a man being homosexual. Groucho (the brother with the mustache) was the fourth brother (of six- the eldest died in infancy, https://en.Wikipedia.org/wiki/Marx_Brothers) and called himself "straight but curved around the edges" (http://everydayheterosexism.blogspot.com/2012/11/the-marx-brothers.html).
Childhood Development
Two other forms of evidence supporting non-social causes of sexual orientation, particularly for males, are related to childhood development and behavior: gender nonconformity and outcomes from attempts to alter gender identity. Children who display gender nonconformity by not following the roles that society expects (Figure \(\PageIndex{4}\)) are more likely to be homosexual as adults. This is a robust finding across cultures, but applies much more strongly to boys than girls. Additionally, "when infant boys are surgically and socially “changed” into girls, their eventual sexual orientation is unchanged (i.e., they remain sexually attracted to females)" (Bailey et al., 2016, page 46). This "natural experiment" is most commonly the result of irreparable damage to the penis, as was the case with David Reimer (see the section on differences of sexual development).
Figure \(\PageIndex{4}\): A two-year-old boy plays nursing his doll. Gender nonconformity in childhood is associated with homosexuality in adulthood.
In Closing: Gender and Sexual Orientation
Although gender (discussed in Section 13.4) and sexual orientation are distinct, some research addresses both concepts concurrently. A review of biological origins for both sexual orientation and gender identity concludes that "multidisciplinary evidence reveals that ... sexual orientation and gender identity are conferred ... during the first half of pregnancy" and that "multiple layers of evidence confirm that sexual orientation and gender identity are as biological, innate and immutable as the other traits conferred during that critical time in gestation" (O'Hanlan et al., 2018). In other words, you cannot change an individual's sexual orientation and/or gender identity any more easily than you can change whether or not they developed a standard human body form with two arms and legs, two eyes and ears, a nose, mouth and typical internal organs.
Summary
Sexual orientation is an individual's emotional and sexual attraction to a particular sex or gender, including their sense of identity based on those attractions and related behaviors. A person may exhibit sexual fluidity (with attractions and behaviors changing due to circumstances), but sexual orientations tend to be relatively stable over the lifespan. Some argue that sexual attraction is primarily driven by reproduction, but empirical studies suggest that pleasure is the primary force behind our sex drive, and that reproduction has relatively little to do with it.
Arguing that dividing people into homosexual and heterosexual did not capture the breadth of human sexual orientation, Kinsey created a continuum with a seven-point scale called the Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating Scale. Zero is exclusively heterosexual, three is bisexual (with equal attractions to the same and opposite sexes), and six is exclusively homosexual. Subsequent research has supported this idea that sexual orientation ranges along a continuum. Representative studies of US and other Western populations report a 3.5% prevalence of non-heterosexual (gay/lesbian/bisexual) orientation. However, how a respondent is asked to report their sexual orientation produces very different results: using the Kinsey Scale, 18% to 39% of Europeans and Americans identify as somewhere between heterosexual and homosexual, whereas only 0.5% to 1.9% of respondents identify as non-heterosexual when researchers force individuals to respond using only two categories. While the percentage of US adults identifying as LGBT (lesbian/gay/bisexual/transgender) remains stable in older-aged cohorts, younger people are more likely to identify as LGBT than previous generations. Examples of terms currently used to identify sexual orientation include heterosexuality (attraction to the opposite sex/gender), homosexuality or same-sex attraction (attraction to the same sex/gender), bisexuality (attraction to two sexes/genders), polysexuality (attraction to multiple sexes/genders), pansexuality (attraction to all sexes/genders), and asexuality (no sexual attraction to any sex/gender).
Individuals are usually aware of their sexual orientation between middle childhood and early adolescence, and sexual activity is not necessary for this recognition. There is no scientific consensus regarding the root cause(s) of sexual orientation, but it is clearly not a conscious "choice" for either heterosexual or non-heterosexual individuals. Research has examined many possible biological, developmental, social, and cultural influences on sexual orientation. Social causes (such as having homosexual parents, recruitment by homosexual adults, and experiencing disordered parenting) have only weak support and many confounding factors. Nonsocial causes (such as genetics, prenatal hormone exposure, the fraternal-birth-order effect, and behavior differences during childhood development) have much more supporting evidence.
Genetic influence on sexual orientation is explored using twin studies. Concordance for sexual orientation is highest for monozygotic twins; and concordance rates for dizygotic twins, siblings, and randomly-selected pairs do not differ. Since concordance rates are most similar for monozygotic twins, this indicates that "nature" (genetics) influences the expression of sexual orientation. However, since monozygotic twins are not 100% concordant, "nurture" (the environment and individual experiences) also plays a role. During prenatal development, excess exposure to androgens for females and deficient exposure to androgens for males has also been theorized as explaining non-heterosexual orientation. Homosexual males tend to have more older brothers than heterosexual males, termed the "fraternal-birth-order effect. One proposed mechanism for this is that the mother creates antibodies against proteins on her developing son's Y chromosome, and that those antibodies may prevent the normal functioning of subsequent sons' Y chromosomes.
Childhood gender nonconformity and outcomes from attempts to alter gender identity also support non-social causes of sexual orientation (particularly for males). Boys who do not follow expected societal roles are more likely to be homosexual as adults. Additionally, when boys are “changed” into girls during childhood (often as a result of irreparable damage to the penis), they remain sexually attracted to women in adulthood. Both sexual orientation and gender identity are determined, at least in part, during prenatal development, and as such are not easily altered.
References
Note: These references are specifically those added to the content of this page by Naomi Bahm and do not include citations from the outside sources used. Refer to the text attributions to locate citations for articles from other sources.
Bailey, J.M., Vasey, P.L., Diamond, L.M., Breedlove, S.M., Vilain, E., & Epprecht, M. (2016). Sexual orientation, controversy, and science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(2), 45-101. doi: 10.1177/1529100616637616
Blanchard, R., Cantor, J., Bogaert, A., Breedlove, S.M., & Ellis, L. (2006). Interaction of fraternal birth order and handedness in the development of male homosexuality. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 405-414. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.09.002
Bogaert, A. (2007). Extreme right-handedness, older brothers, and sexual orientation in men. Neuropsychology, 21(1), 141-148. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.1.141
Dimock, M. (2019). Defining generations: Where Millennials end and Generation Z begins. Pew Research Center (January 17), https://pewrsr.ch/2szqtJz
Gates, G.J. (2017). LGBT data collection amid social and demographic shifts of the US LGBT community. American Journal of Public Health, 107 (8), 1220-1222. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2017.303927
O'Hanlan, K.A., Gordon, J.C., & Sullivan, M.W. (2018). Biological origins of sexual orientation and gender identity: Impact on health. Gynecologic Oncology, 149, 33-42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygyno.2017.11.014
Text from above two sources pieced together with some modifications, transitions and additional images and content (particularly in the sections A Continuum of Sexual Orientation, and Development and Origins of Sexual Orientation) added by Naomi I. Gribneau Bahm, PhD., Psychology Professor at Cosumnes River College, Sacramento, CA.