Skip to main content
Social Sci LibreTexts

6.1: Primate Ecology and Behavior

  • Page ID
    136404
  • \( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \) \( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)\(\newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\) \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\) \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \(\newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\) \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\) \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)\(\newcommand{\AA}{\unicode[.8,0]{x212B}}\)

    Karin Enstam Jaffe, Ph.D., Sonoma State University

    Learning Objectives
    • Describe the behavioral variation that exists within the Primate Order and how primate behavior and morphology are influenced by diet, predation, and other ecological factors.
    • Explain why primates live in groups.
    • Contrast male and female reproductive and parental investment strategies.
    • Describe the ways in which primates communicate.
    alt
    Figure 6.1b A juvenile capuchin monkey in Serra da Capivara, Brazil, uses a stone as a tool to open a seed.
    alt
    Figure 6.1a A female Japanese macaque nursing her infant.

    Nonhuman primates (from now on simply referred to as “primates”) are our closest living relatives, and their behavior is often strikingly similar to our own. If you’ve ever seen a female monkey at your local zoo cooing over her newborn baby (Figure 6.1a) or watched a video of a tufted capuchin monkey using rocks as a hammer and anvil to crack open a nut to access the edible kernel inside (Figure 6.1b), then you know how interesting they can be.

    alt
    Figure 6.2 Map of Kenya with Laikipia District, where the author conducted her fieldwork, highlighted.

    I have been fascinated by primates since I was a young child. In the summer of 1996, I went with my dissertation advisor, Dr. Lynne A. Isbell, to her field site in Laikipia, Kenya (Figure 6.2) with the intention of studying the play behavior of juvenile patas monkeys. One day we were following our patas study group when several females and juveniles began giving high-pitched “nyow” alarm calls. I was awestruck as I watched the entire group take off at breakneck speed. Patas monkeys are, after all, the fastest primate, capable of running 20 miles per hour for short distances. It did not even occur to me that they had sounded an alarm and then run away from something —until my advisor pointed to the lioness hidden in the grass at the base of a tree. (We slowly backed away and got in our car.) My research interests changed in that moment: I wanted to study primate antipredator behavior, the strategies primates use to escape from predators. I would spend two years at that same field site collecting data on anti-predator behavior of patas monkeys and vervets, two closely related species who occupy different habitats. Patas monkeys (Figure 6.3a) live far from rivers, in habitats composed of short trees spaced far apart (Figure 6.3b). These trees have little to no overlapping canopy, so climbing one to escape a lion in pursuit can result in a literal dead end. In contrast, vervets (Figure 6.4a) spend most of their time along rivers, with access to tall trees with overlapping canopies (Figure 6.4b) that provide good escape routes from terrestrial predators. But they also venture into patas habitats, the short trees with canopies that do not overlap. I wanted to know: How would the structure of these habitats affect the responses of vervets and patas monkeys to alarm calls that signal the approach of a terrestrial predator like a lion? Not surprisingly, when vervets are near the river, they climb the tall trees to seek refuge from such predators. But not patas monkeys. These “cheetahs of the primate world” are more likely to take off running (as I had seen them do that summer), even bypassing nearby trees. Their physical adaptations for speed, like their long legs, combined with the lack of arboreal escape routes, makes fleeing on the ground their best option. But what do vervets do when they are away from the river and the safety of their tall trees? Is their behavior “hard-wired” so that their response to an alarm call is the same, regardless of the habitat? Or do they assess key aspects of their habitat, like tree height and canopy cover, and alter their behavior? Although they cannot run as fast, when they hear an alarm call they run back toward the river, by-passing the short trees, just like the patas do (Enstam and Isbell 2002). The implication is clear: these monkeys, our close relatives, with their highly developed intelligence and ability to learn, do assess key features of their habitat and use this information to alter their behavior and maximize their chance of escape.

    alt
    Figure 6.3a and 6.3b A female patas monkey with infant (left). A patas habitat in Laikipia, Kenya (right).
    alt
    Figure 6.4a and 6.4b A female vervet (left). A vervet habitat in Laikipia, Kenya (right).
    alt
    Figure 6.5 The author observing patas monkeys in Laikipia, Kenya.

    The branch of science that focuses on the study of primate behavior is called primatology, and people, like myself, who study primates (Figure 6.5) are called primatologists. Primatologists come from many different disciplines and study primate behavior for different reasons. Biologists study primates as examples of evolutionary theories like natural selection or parental investment. Primate intelligence is of interest to psychologists who want to learn more about the underlying cognitive principles involved in deceptive or cooperative behavior and to linguists interested in the principles of communication and language. Ecologists studying conservation issues examine how primates are affected by deforestation, poaching, or illegal animal trade. Biological anthropologists, like myself, who study primates are interested in their social complexity and ecological and behavioral variation. Because both humans and most nonhuman primates live in groups, biological anthropologists study primates to better understand the evolution of social behavior and its costs and benefits. Because primates are our closest living relatives, we study them to gain insights into how our human ancestors may have behaved as well as to better understand our own behavior.

    About the Author

    Karin Enstam Jaffe

    Sonoma State University, karin.jaffe@sonoma.edu

    alt
    Karin Enstam Jaffe

    Dr. Karin Enstam Jaffe has loved primates since she was five years old. As an undergraduate at U.C. San Diego, she participated in projects studying orangutans, langurs, and Mona monkeys. She earned her Ph.D. in Anthropology from U.C. Davis studying vervet and patas monkey anti-predator behavior. Dr. Jaffe has over 20 years of experience studying primate behavior in Kenya and Grenada and at the San Diego Zoo, San Francisco Zoo, and Safari West Wildlife Preserve in Sonoma County, California. She has been a faculty member in the Anthropology Department at Sonoma State University since August 2002. A dedicated teacher-scholar, Dr. Jaffe has won several teaching, scholarship, and mentoring awards, including SSU’s Excellence in Teaching Award, Educational Experience Enhancement Award, and the President’s Excellence in Scholarship Award. In addition to teaching, she is a Research Associate at Oakland Zoo, where she has been involved in behavioral enrichment research involving ring-tailed lemurs, chimpanzees, and sun bears, as well as a study of the social network of hamadryas baboons.

    For Further Exploration

    Fossey, Dian. 1983. Gorillas in the Mist. London: Hodder & Stoughton.

    Goodall, Jane. 1971. In the Shadow of Man. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Rowe, Noel, and Marc Myers, eds. 2016. All the World’s Primates. Charleston, Rhode Island: Pogonias Press.

    Strier, Karen B. 2017. Primate Behavioral Ecology. 5th ed. New York: Routledge.

    Primate Info Net (http://pin.primate.wisc.edu/) is an information service of the National Primate Research Center at the University of Wisconsin, Madison. It includes Primate Factsheets, primate news and publications, a list of primate-related jobs, and an international directory of primatology, among other information.

    Primate Specialist Group (http://www.primate-sg.org/) is a collection of scientists and conservationists who work in dozens of African, Asian, and Latin American nations to promote research on primate conservation.

    References

    Aich, H., R. Moos-Heilen, and E. Zimmermann. 1990. “Vocalizations of Adult Gelada Baboons (Theropithecus gelada): Acoustic Structure and Behavioural Context.” Folia Primatologica 55 (3–4): 109–132.

    Arlet, Małgorzata E., Freerk Molleman, and Colin Chapman. 2007. “Indications for Female Mate Choice in Grey-Cheeked Mangabeys Lophocebus albigena johnstoni in Kibale National Park, Uganda.” Acta Ethologica 10 (2): 89–95.

    Baker, Andrew J., James M. Dietz, and Devra G. Kleiman. 1993. “Behavioural Evidence for Monopolization of Paternity in Multi-Male Groups of Golden Lion Tamarins.” Animal Behaviour 46 (6): 1,091–1,103.

    Bell, Sarah A. 2017. “Galdikas, Birute.” In The International Encyclopedia of Primatology, Volume A–G, edited by Agustín Fuentes, 445–446. Malden, Massachusetts: John Wiley & Sons.

    Boesch, Christophe. 2002. “Cooperative Hunting Roles Among Taï Chimpanzees.” Human Nature 13 (1): 27–46.

    Boesch, Christophe, and Hedwige Boesch. 1989. “Hunting Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees in the Taï National Park.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 78 (4): 547–573.

    Boinski, S. 1992. “Olfactory Communication among Costa Rican Squirrel Monkeys: A Field Study.” Folia Primatologica 59 (3): 127–136.

    Borries, Carola, Kristin Launhardt, Cornelia Epplen, Jörg T. Epplen, and Paul Winkler. 1999. “Males as Infant Protectors in Hanuman Langurs (Presbytis entellus) Living in Multimale Groups: Defence Pattern, Paternity, and Sexual Behaviour.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 46 (5): 350–356.

    Chapman, Colin A., and Carlos A. Peres. 2001. “Primate Conservation in the New Millennium: The Role of Scientists.” Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 10 (1): 16–33.

    Cheney, D. L., and R. M. Seyfarth. 1987. “The Influence of Intergroup Competition on the Survival and Reproduction of Female Vervet Monkeys.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 21 (6): 375–386.

    Chism, J. B., T. E. Rowell, and D. Olson. 1984. “Life History Patterns of Female Patas Monkeys.” In Female Primates: Studies of Women Primatologists, edited by Meredith Small, 175–190. New York: A.R. Liss.

    Clarke, Margaret R., Kenneth E. Glander, and Evan L. Zucker. 1998. “Infant-Nonmother Interactions in Free-Ranging Howlers (Alouatta palliata) in Costa Rica. International Journal of Primatology 19 (3): 451–472.

    Crockett, Carolyn M., and Theresa Pope. 1988. “Inferring Patterns of Aggression from Red Howler Monkey Injuries.” American Journal of Primatology 15 (4): 289–308.

    Díaz-Muñoz, Samuel L. 2011. “Paternity and Relatedness in a Polyandrous Nonhuman Primate: Testing Adaptive Hypotheses of Male Reproductive Cooperation.” Animal Behaviour 82 (3): 563–571.

    Digby, Leslie J., Stephen F. Ferrari, and Wendy Saltzman. (2007) 2011. “Callitrichines: The Role of Competition in Cooperatively Breeding Species.” In Primates in Perspective, edited by Christina J. Campbell, AugustÍn Fuentes, Katherine C. MacKinnon, Simon K. Bearder, and Rebecca M. Stumpf, 91–10. Second edition, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Eckardt, Winnie, and Klaus Zuberbühler. 2004. “Cooperation and Competition in Two Forest Monkeys.” Behavioral Ecology 15 (3): 400–411.

    Enstam, Karin L., and Lynne A. Isbell. 2002. “Comparison of Responses to Alarm Calls by Patas (Erythrocebus patas) and Vervet (Cercopithecus aethiops) Monkeys in Relation to Habitat Structure.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 119 (1): 3–14.

    Enstam, Karin L., Lynne A. Isbell, and Thomas W. de Maar. 2002. “Male Demography, Female Mating Behavior, and Infanticide in Wild Patas Monkeys (Erythrocebus patas).” International Journal of Primatology. 23 (1): 85–104.

    Fedigan, Linda Marie. 1990. “Vertebrate Predation in Cebus capucinus: Meat Eating in a Neotropical Monkey.” Folia Primatologica 54 (3–4): 196–205.

    Fischer, Julia, Kurt Hammerschmidt, Dorothy L. Cheney, and Robert M. Seyfarth. 2008. “Acoustic Features of Female Chacma Baboon Barks.” Ethology 107 (1): 33–54.

    Fossey, Dian. 1983. Gorillas in the Mist. London: Hodder & Stoughton.

    Gardner, Charlie J., Patrick Radolalaina, Mahandry Rajerison, and Harry W. Greene. 2015. “Cooperative Rescue and Predator Fatality Involving a Group-Living Strepsirrhine, Coquerel’s Sifaka (Propithecus coquereli), and a Madagascar Ground Boa (Acrantophis madagascariensis).” Primates 56 (2): 127–129.

    Gautier-Hion, Annie, René Quris, and Jean-Pierre Gautier. 1983. “Monospecific vs. Polyspecific Life: A Comparative Study of Foraging and Antipredator Tactics in a Community of Cercopithecus Monkeys.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 12 (4): 325–335.

    Harcourt, A. H., P. H. Harvey, S. G. Larson, and R. V. Short. 1981. “Testis Weight, Body Weight and Breeding System in Primates.” Nature 293 (5,827): 55–57.

    Henzi, Peter, and Louise Barrett. 2003. “Evolutionary Ecology, Sexual Conflict, and Behavioral Differentiation among Baboon Populations.” Evolutionary Anthropology 12 (5): 217–230.

    Isbell, Lynne A. 1991. “Contest and Scramble Competition: Patterns of Female Aggression and Ranging Behavior Among Primates.” Behavioral Ecology 2 (2): 143–155.

    Isbell, Lynne A., Dorothy L. Cheney, and Robert M. Seyfarth. 1990. “Costs and Benefits of Home Range Shifts Among Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) in Amboseli National Park, Kenya.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 27 (5): 351–358.

    Isbell, Lynne A., and Dirk Van Vuren. 1996. “Differential Costs of Locational and Social Dispersal and Their Consequences for Female Group-Living Primates.” Behaviour 133 (1–2): 1–36.

    Jolly, Alison. 1966. Lemur Behavior: A Madagascar Field Study. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Lutz, Clayton L., Duane R. Diefenbach, and Christopher S. Rosenberry. 2016. “Proximate Influences on Female Dispersal in White-Tailed Deer.” Journal of Wildlife Management 80 (7): 1,218–1,226.

    Marty, Pascal R., Maria A. van Noordwijk, Michael Heistermann, Erik P. Willems, Lynda P. Dunkel, Manuela Cadilek, Muhammad Agil, and Tony Weingrill. 2015. “Endocrinological Correlates of Male Bimaturism in Wild Bornean Orangutans.” American Journal of Primatology 77 (11): 1,170–1,178.

    McGrew, W. C. 1998. “Culture in Nonhuman Primates?” Annual Review of Anthropology 27 (1): 301–328.

    Mertl-Millhollen, Anne S. 1988. “Olfactory Demarcation of Territorial but Not Home Range Boundaries by Lemur catta.” Folia Primatologica 50 (3–4): 175–187.

    National Geographic. n.d. “Chimps and Tools.” Accessed July 25, 2019. https://video.nationalgeographic.com/video/00000144-0a1e-d3cb-a96c-7b1fadbd0000

    National Geographic. n.d. “Meditative Snow Monkeys Hang Out in Hot Springs.” Accessed July 25, 2019. https://video.nationalgeographic.com/video/short-film-showcase/00000149-d415-de71-a9eb-dc9539210000

    Nekaris, K. Anne-Isola, Richard S. Moore, E. Johanna Rode, and Bryan G. Fry. 2013. “Mad, Bad, and Dangerous to Know: The Biochemistry, Ecology, and Evolution of Slow Loris Venom.” The Journal of Venomous Animals and Toxins Including Tropical Diseases 19 (1): 21.

    Peres, Carlosa. 1992. “Prey-Capture Benefits in a Mixed-Species Group of Amazonian Tamarins, Saguinus fuscicollis and S. mystax.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 31 (5): 339–347. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00177774.

    Raboy, Becky E., Gustavo R. Canale, and James M. Dietz. 2008. “Ecology of Callithrix kuhlii and a Review of Eastern Brazilian Marmosets.” International Journal of Primatology 29 (2): 449–467.

    Ransom, Timothy W. 1981. Beach Troop of the Gombe. East Brunswick, New Jersey: Bucknell University Press.

    Santana, Sharlene E., Jessica Lynch Alfaro, and Michael E. Alfaro. 2012. “Adaptive Evolution of Facial Colour Patterns in Neotropical Primates.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279 (1,736): 2,204–2,211.

    Schön Ybarra, M. A. 1986. “Loud Calls of Adult Male Red Howling Monkeys (Alouatta seniculus).” Folia Primatologica 47 (4): 204–216.

    Schwitzer, Christoph, Russell A. Mittermeier, Anthony B. Rylands, Federica Chiozza, Elizabeth A. Williamson, Elizabeth J. Macfie, Janette Wallis, and Alison Cotton. 2017. Primates in Peril: The World’s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2016–2018. Arlington, VA: IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), Conservation International (CI), and Bristol Zoological Society.

    Sekulic, Ranka. 1982. “The Function of Howling in Red Howler Monkeys (Alouatta seniculus).” Behaviour 81 (1): 38–54.

    Setchell, Joanna M., Tessa Smith, E. Jean Wickings, and Leslie A. Knapp. 2008. “Social Correlates of Testosterone and Ornamentation in Male Mandrills.” Hormones and Behavior 54 (3): 365–372.

    Setchell, Joanna M., E. Jean Wickings, and Leslie A. Knapp. 2006. “Signal Content of Red Facial Coloration in Female Mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx).” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences273 (1599): 2395–2400.

    Seyfarth, R. M., D. L. Cheney, and P. Marler. 1980a. “Monkey Responses to Three Different Alarm Calls: Evidence of Predator Classification and Semantic Communication.” Science 210 (4,471): 801–803.

    Seyfarth, Robert M., Dorothy L. Cheney, and Peter Marler. 1980b. “Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic Communication in a Free-Ranging Primate.” Animal Behaviour 28 (4): 1,070–1,094.

    Silk, Joan B., Susan C. Alberts, and Jeanne Altmann. 2003. “Social Bonds of Female Baboons Enhance Infant Survival.” Science 302 (5,648): 1,231–1,234.

    Smith, David Glenn. 1994. “Male Dominance and Reproductive Success in a Captive Group of Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta).” Behaviour 129 (3): 225–242.

    Smithsonian Channel. June 9, 2017. “Why These Vegetarian Monkeys Have Sharp Predator Teeth.” Accessed July 25, 2019. https://www.youtube.com/watch?time_continue=145&v=aC6iYj_EBjY

    Stanford, Craig B. 2017. “Goodall, Jane.” In The International Encyclopedia of Primatology, Volume A–G, edited by Agustín Fuentes, 471–472. Malden, Massachusetts: John Wiley & Sons.

    Stewart, Kelly. 2017. “Fossey, Dian.” In The International Encyclopedia of Primatology, Volume A–G, edited by Agustín Fuentes, 432–433. Malden, Massachusetts: John Wiley & Sons.

    Stumpf, R. M., M. Emery Thompson, M. N. Muller, and R. W. Wrangham. 2009. “The Context of Female Dispersal in Kanyawara Chimpanzees.” Behaviour 146 (4–5): 629–656.

    Swedell, Larissa, and Amy Schreier. 2006. “Male Aggression toward Females in Hamadryas Baboons: Conditioning, Coercion, and Control.” In Sexual Coercion in Primates and Humans, edited by Martin N. Muller and Richard W. Wrangham, 244–268. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Terborgh, John. 1984. Five New World Primates: A Study in Comparative Ecology. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Trivers, Robert L. 1972. “Parental Investment and Sexual Selection.” In Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, 1871–1971, edited by Bernard Campbell, 136–179. Chicago: Aldine.

    van Schaik, Carel P., and Peter M. Kappeler. 1997. “Infanticide Risk and the Evolution of Male-Female Association in Primates.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 264 (1,388): 1,687–1,694.

    Whiten, Andrew. 2011. “The Scope of Culture in Chimpanzees, Humans and Ancestral Apes.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 366 (1,567): 997–1,007.

    Whitten, Patricia L. 1983. “Diet and Dominance among Female Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops).” American Journal of Primatology 5 (2): 139–159.

    Wiens, Frank, and Annette Zitzmann. 2003. “Social Structure of the Solitary Slow Loris Nycticebus coucang (Lorisidae).” Journal of Zoology 261 (1): 35–46.

    Zuberbühler, Klaus, David Jenny, and Redouan Bshary. 1999. “The Predator Deterrence Function of Primate Alarm Calls.” Ethology 105 (6): 477–490.

    Zuberbühler, Klaus, Ronald Noë, and Robert M. Seyfarth. 1997. “Diana Monkey Long-Distance Calls: Messages for Conspecifics and Predators.” Animal Behaviour 53 (3): 589–604.

    Acknowledgements

    The author is grateful to the editors for the opportunity to contribute to this open-source textbook. She thanks Stephanie Etting for her encouragement and support during the writing of this chapter. Her suggestions, along with comments made by two anonymous reviewers on an earlier version of this chapter, improved the final version considerably. Finally, she thanks all the primatologists who came before her, especially her advisor, Lynne A. Isbell, for their tireless efforts to understand the behavior and ecology of the living primates. Without their work, this chapter would not have been possible.

    Figure Attributions

    Figure 6.1a Snow monkey baby milk time by Daisuke tashiro is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.1b Stone tool use by a capuchin monkey by Tiago Falótico is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.2 Laikipia location map by Nairobi123 is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.3a Female patas monkey with infant by Karin Enstam Jaffe is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.3b Patas habitat in Laikipia, Kenya by Karin Enstam Jaffe is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.3a Monkey (27996706462) by Martie Swart from Johannesburg, South Africa is under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.4b Vervet habitat in Laikipia, Kenya by Karin Enstam Jaffe is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.5 Karin Enstam Jaffe observing patas monkeys in Laikipia, Kenya by Rebecca Chancellor is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.6a Spectral Tarsier Tarsius tarsier (7911549768) by Bernard DUPONT from FRANCE has been modified (cropped) and is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.6b Mountain gorilla (Gorilla beringei beringei) eating by Charles J Sharp creator QS:P170,Q54800218 is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.7 Food abundance and food scarcity by Karin Enstam Jaffe original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.8 Food distribution patterns by Karin Enstam Jaffe original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.9 Food distribution patterns of folivores, frugivores, and insectivores a derivative work by Karin Enstam Jaffe original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License. [Includes Green fruit tree, Lemon tree, and Ladybug image, all from publicdomainvectors.org designated to the public domain (CC0)].

    Figure 6.10 Termite mound-Tanzania by Vierka Maráková, Slovakia (Krokodild) has been designated to the public domain (CC0).

    Figure 6.11a Patas Monkey Jr by alex roberts from San Francisco, USA is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.11b Chlorocebus pygerythrus by Xlandfair at English Wikipedia has been designated to the public domain (CC0).

    Figure 6.11c Male Olive Baboon 2 by ryan harvey from Portland, OR is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.11d Komba ušatá2 by Petr Hamerník is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.12 Muriquis 3 by Mônica Imbuzeiro is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.13 Examples of primate communities table original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Karin Enstam Jaffe is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.14a Lion tailed macaque eating Lizard by Prasadchangarath is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.14b Gombe Stream NP Beute by Ikiwaner is used under a GNU License.

    Figure 6.15a Leopard africa by JanErkamp at the English Wikipedia is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.15b Jaguar (Panthera onca palustris) male Rio Negro 2 by Charles J Sharp creator QS:P170,Q54800218 is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.15c Male Tiger Ranthambhore (3646793728) by Koshy Koshy is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.15d Fosa fotovideo by Bertal is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.15e Martial eagle (Polemaetus bellicosus) by Charles J Sharp creator QS:P170,Q54800218 is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.15f Harpy Eagle with wings lifted by Jonathan Wilkins is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.15g Python natalensis G. J. Alexander by Graham J. Alexander, University of the Witwatersrand has been designated to the public domain (CC0).

    Figure 6.16 Slow Loris by Jmiksanek is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.17 Monkey group by Natureloverperson is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.18a Deforestation near Bukit Tiga Puluh NP by Aidenvironment (1998) is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.18b Lemur poaching 004 by author who does not wish to be named for safety reasons is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.19a Dierenpark Emmen baboon (2679944324) by robin bos from heerenveen, the Netherlands is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.19b Male Gelada Baboon, Chenek, Simien Mountains (6190375633) by Rod Waddington from Kergunyah, Australia has been modified (cropped) and is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.20a Azy (orangutan) at the Indianapolis Zoo by Tom Britt is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.20b Orangutan male (15433476577) by Russ is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.21a Emperor Tamarin with babies sf by Brocken Inaglory is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.21b Family of Common Marmoset – REGUA – Brazil MG 9480 (12930855765) by Francesco Veronesi from Italy is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.22 Social and Mating Systems by Karin Enstam Jaffe original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.23a Jane Goodall HK by Jeekc is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.23b US-223658 Dian Fossey (2925879356) by Mary-Lynn is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.23c Dr Birute Galdikas by Simon Fraser University – University Communications is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.24a Sexual swelling in female Hamadryas baboon by Mamoritai is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.24b Hamadryas baboon at Giza Zoo by Hatem Moushir 36 by Hatem Moushir is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.25 Mandrill 08 by SuperJew is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.26 BabouinGeladaAuReveil by BluesyPete is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.27 Mandrill 09 by SuperJew is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.28a Uakari by Coada dragos is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.28b Ateles belzebuth (White-bellied spider monkey) 2 by Ewa is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.

    Figure 6.29 Lemur catta 004 by Alex Dunkel (Visionholder) is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.

    Figure 6.30a Yakushima macaques grooming each other by Grendelkhan is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.30b Tufted capuchin monkeys grooming session III by Adrian Soldati is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.30c Baboons Wunania 012018 by Kim Toogood is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.30d Black-and-white ruffed lemur 03 by Mattis2412 has been designated to the public domain (CC0 1.0).

    Figure 6.31 Pan troglodytes, tool use in Senegal by J. D. Pruetz, P. Bertolani, K. Boyer Ontl, S. Lindshield, M. Shelley, and E. G. Wessling is used under a CC BY 4.0 License.

    Figure 6.32 Jigokudani hotspring in Nagano Japan 001 by Yosemite is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.


    1. Smithsonian Channel. June 9, 2017. “Why These Vegetarian Monkeys Have Sharp Predator Teeth.” Accessed July 25, 2019. https://www.youtube.com/watch?time_continue=145&v=aC6iYj_EBjY
    2. National Geographic. n.d. “Chimps and Tools.” Accessed July 25, 2019. https://video.nationalgeographic.com/video/00000144-0a1e-d3cb-a96c-7b1fadbd0000
    3. National Geographic. n.d. “Meditative Snow Monkeys Hang Out in Hot Springs.” Accessed July 25, 2019. https://video.nationalgeographic.com/video/short-film-showcase/00000149-d415-de71-a9eb-dc9539210000

    6.1: Primate Ecology and Behavior is shared under a CC BY-NC license and was authored, remixed, and/or curated by LibreTexts.

    • Was this article helpful?