Children acquire language at their own pace, mastering the components of language as they develop. The entirety of their developmental journey is filled with cues that they give to and receive from others. Children’s individual development has biological, environmental, and contextual influences. This voyage is profoundly different for each child based on their language experiences, which impart meaning to their attempts at communication. Even with individual differences, there are some predictable patterns. Language development occurs as children develop receptive and expressive language in ways that foster social communication, and there are identifiable stages or windows of growth.
Receptive Language
Receptive language can be defined as “how we receive information and understand words and their meaning” (Virginia Department of Education, 2021). For the purpose of this textbook, receptive language is the ability to understand information transmitted by others. This includes understanding the words or sentences one hears as well as the meaning of what is communicated through gestures or signs, or in written form. Receptive language implies comprehension of the material being received, and it develops prior to expressive language (Bloom & Lahey, 1978). We generally understand more words than we use. All humans have more receptive than productive vocabulary, whether using their home or a new language (Cleci-Murcia & Oshtain, 2001). Receptive language requires knowledge of the meaning of the gestures or words being communicated (McGuiness, 2005).
There is some evidence that we do respond to sounds prenatally, indicating that receptive language begins before birth. Babies in utero may sometimes respond to a loud noise, suggesting that they can hear (Marno et al., 2016). In addition to sound, there is evidence that babies are learning patterns of speech and react in the womb by kicking or demonstrating elevated heart rate (Karmiloff & Karmiloff-Smith, 2001). This early exposure results in a preference for language or native tongue of the mother in a newborn (Marno et al, 2016). Babies show a change in their heart rate when a speaker uses their mother’s native tongue compared to a speaker using a different language (Minai et al, 2017). Newborns even change their thumb-sucking rhythm and pattern in response to the language and the speakers they hear (Karmiloff & Karmiloff-Smith, 2001).
Parents engage infants in language that differs from other points in the life cycle. They speak to babies using high-pitched voices with exaggerated pronunciation and movements (Koester & Lahti-Harper, 2010). This child directed speech is also referred to as motherese or parentese, although it is used by other adults besides parents. Hearing infants respond to child directed speech with greater alertness and exclamatory sounds. Parents who use sign language also demonstrate a form of child directed speech with exaggerated movements and facial expressions (Masataka, 1996). Child directed speech is purposeful, and demonstrates attentiveness toward the infant (Koester & Lahti-Harper, 2010), but it is impossible to truly say if it is occurring because the parent initiates or the child elicits the exaggerated response. Either way, it is clear that there is something innate about our desire and capacity for connection which is reinforced in our early communication attempts. In other words, we bring our preadapted language capacity into the world, and the environment in turn shapes our development in these areas.
During infancy the child amasses an understanding of the sounds required for the language they are exposed to. Children are born with the capacity to produce and distinguish the sounds required for all languages, though they prefer their home language (Marno et al, 2016). Within one year, their brains begin to strengthen the connections they need to support their home language, and prune away or disuse the connections that do not help them meet their communicative goals. By 10 months of age, their ability to distinguish among sounds that are not in their home language has diminished (Conboy & Kuhl, 2011), though this loss does not occur in their native tongue. This process of distinguishing the sounds of one’s native language strengthens the ability to learn language overall (Kuhl et al, 2005). Receptive language is developmental, and benchmark timeframes overlap each other as seen in the tables below.
Developmental Progression: Receptive Language
Refer to the early learning standards in your state to see the progression of receptive language skills. To review early learning language standards for receptive langauge in Wisconsin, visit Wisconsin Model Early Learning Standards.
The table below represents the Virginia Early Learning & Development Standards.
Table 3.2.1 “Developmental Progression: Receptive Language” Content is from Virginia’s Early Learning & Development Standards (ELDS): Birth-Five Learning Guidelines. It is used with permission of the creators and is not an open education resource covered by a Creative Commons License.
Developmental Progression
CLLD1.2 Using Vocabulary and Nonverbal Cues to Communicate
Early Infancy
0-8 months
Responds to speaker by turning head or shifting eye gaze (CLLD1.1a)
Responds to tone or voice changes (CLLD1.1b)
Directs attention to sounds or object pointed at/to (CLLD1.1c)
Responds by looking when name is called (CLLD1.1d)
Later Infancy
6-14 months
Follows simple requests paired with gestures (CLLD1.1e)
Begins to respond through sounds or gestures to others’ questions (CLLD1.1f)
Makes different sounds to get attention (CLLD1.1g)
Understands and responds to simple commands and gestures (e.g., shared eye gaze/contact; may be influenced by cultural expectations) (CLLD1.1h)
Recognizes common objects when named (e.g., cup) (CLLD1.1i)
Begins to show understanding of approximately 50 words (CLLD1.1j)
Early Toddler
12-24 Months
Uses sounds or nonverbal cues to respond to oral reading (CLLD1.1k)
Responds to simple requests (CLLD1.1l)
Identifies familiar people, animals, and objects (CLLD1.1m)
Begins to understand nouns (CLLD1.1n)
Begins to understand action words (e.g., hop, jump, press, go, stop) (CLLD1.1o)
Completes a task in response to a request (e.g., pick up your cup, go find your coat) (CLLD1.1p)
Identifies familiar people, animals, and objects (using gestures, words, or sounds) (CLLD1.1q)
Begins to recognize some body parts (CLLD1.1r)
Understands and responds to some pronouns (mine, you, me) (CLLD1.1s)
Later Toddler
22-36 Months
Listens to and attends to spoken language and read-aloud texts (CLLD1.1t)
Follows simple and short directions for routine practices (CLLD1.1u)
Understands and responds to several hundred words or more (CLLD1.1v)
Understands and responds to basic attribute word differences such as personal (I/me/you/it) and possessive (my and mine) pronouns (CLLD1.1)
Early Preschool
34-48 Months
Demonstrates understanding by answering questions (CLLD1.1x)
Follows two-step unrelated directions (CLLD1.1y)
Understands and responds to spatial concepts (e.g., in/out, under, front, behind) (CLLD1.1z)
Later Preschool
44-60 Months
Answers questions and adds comments relevant to the topic (CLLD1.1aa)
Begins to demonstrate understanding of implied messages based on speaker’s tone and/or gestures (e.g., finger on lip and saying “shhhh” means be quiet) (CLLD1.1ab)
Understands and responds to words for descriptive concepts (e.g., hard, square) (CLLD1.1ac)
Follows 2-3 step related directions and some new directions related to familiar and daily routines (CLLD1.1ad)
Demonstrates understanding of communication that includes a variety of complex sentences related to familiar stories, learning activities, and events (e.g., After Clifford the dog came home, he played with Emily Elizabeth.) (CLLD1.1ae)
Productive or Expressive Language
Productive or expressive language is how we use vocabulary to describe objects, actions, and events. In this textbook, expressive language includes the language we produce to communicate our meaning and messages to others. This can occur through the use of verbal or non-verbal sounds, gestures, words, or written language. Expressive language and productive language are interchangeable terms that imply that the child is expressing or producing language for the purpose of having their intent understood. Expressive language develops later than receptive language (Celci-Murcia & Oshtain, 2001) and one’s expressive or productive vocabulary is less than receptive vocabulary. As Celci-Murcia & Oshtain (2001) point out, expressive language usage implies receptive language mastery, but the reverse is not necessarily true. Infants show recognition of common daily words long before they produce their own first words. Children turn their head at hearing their own name before they can say it, and they will eagerly clasp and unclasp their hands upon hearing that they are about to eat a preferred familiar food.
Preverbal stages of language development. Language development occurs along a trajectory that begins with preverbal expression. Productive/expressive language evolves through stages that move from early sounds to word formation. Stark (1986) created a framework for describing expressive language, outlined in the table below.
Stark’s Five-Stage Framework
Table 3.2.2 “Stark’s Five-Stage Framework”. Content is based on Stark R. E. (1986). Prespeech segmental feature development. In Studies in Language Acquisition, Fletcher P, Garman M (eds). Cambridge University Press:149–173.
Stage
Stage Name
Age of Onset
Characteristics
Stage 1
Reflexive crying and vegetative sounds
At birth
Crying
Sneezing
Burping
Stage 2
Cooing and laughter
6-8 weeks
Noises
Stage 3
Vocal play
Between 17 and 30 weeks
Consonant sounds from front of mouth such as muh, puh, nuh, buh, duh,
Some presence of vowel sounds
Noises with mouth such as blowing raspberries or clicking the tongue
Stage 4
Canonical/reduplicative
7-9 months
Syllables in consonant-vowel combinations (ma-ma-ma)
Stage 5
Non-reduplicative babbling
10 months
Various consonant-vowel combinations
Takes on rhythm and pitch of conversation
Our first vocalizations are cries, beginning at birth, and reflect our preverbal language production. While it may begin as an instinct, these early cries let a caregiver know that a baby is distressed and requires care. This certainly is an early form of communication. Over time, children add other types of sounds to their collection whether accidental, such as a sneeze, or intentional such as a grunt. The first stage reflexive crying and vegetative sounds, includes vocal cord vibrations, burping, and blowing, and other sounds made accidentally or on purpose (Stark, 1986).
The second stage begins between 6 and 8 weeks and includes cooing and laughter. Children in this stage sound a bit like owls as they repeat these vowel sounds over and over (Menn & Stoel-Gammon, 2005). During this stage, children begin to laugh and signal their pleasure through use of the social smile that emerges sometime between 4 and 8 weeks.
The third stage, vocal play, emerges when children are between 17 and 30 weeks old. In vocal play children make vowel sounds and other sounds including consonants or friction sounds, such as by blowing raspberries or clicking their tongue. In this stage, children also add consonant sounds to their cooing, called babbling (Fagan, 2015). These preliminary attempts, called marginal babbling, see children producing some consonants with vowel sounds, but without true syllables. A child may produce consonants at the front of the mouth, such as “buh”, but might not necessarily produce the vowel sound in a clearly distinguishable way.
The fourth stage,canonical or reduplicative babbling emerges when children are between 7 and 9 months old, and is distinguished from the previous stage by distinct syllable sounds. Babbling becomes more complex over time, taking on closer approximations of words, and the rhythms and pitches of the home language (Lipkind et al, 2013). This more complex version of babbling is called variegated babbling (Gratier, et al, 2015) or canonical babbling (Vihman & Greenlee, 1987).
The fifth stage, nonreduplicative babbling is the production of sounds that sound very much like language, even if the words do not match. Children use distinctive consonant combinations, and the rhythm and pitch of the language sounds like speech. In addition, parents and children engage in communicative exchanges called protoconversation (Gratier, et al, 2015). Protoconversation occurs when the baby coos or babbles and the parent responds to the baby. Even though the conversation may not communicate a particular meaning, or could appear to be nonsensical, it is actually very helpful as children are hearing language sounds, learning about conversational turn taking, and learning that someone will listen to them.
Holophrases/single words. Holophrases/single words appear as children move from babbling to producing more organized and familiar sounds. The sounds that children use for babbling, may be sounds that they hear in language and can mimic, but may also represent their first words (Menn & Stoel-Gammon, 2005). This may occur even if it is hard to recognize the word from an adult perspective. Examples of this may be a sibling’s name that is hard to pronounce such as “La-la” for “Greta” or “Go” for the more difficult to pronounce “John.” In addition, a child may use a word to denote a whole phrase or intention they are meaning to communicate. For example, “Fue-da” (an approximation of “afuera” which means “outside” in Spanish) may communicate that the child wants to go outside. In a seminal study of parents and children from three cities, Bates et al, (1994), project that children may utter their first word as early as eight months, increasing to a vocabulary of approximately 10 words at the end of the first year, and 312 words by the time they are at the end of the second year. Productive language includes signs and gestures, and as with receptive language development, has a trajectory that allows for individual differences.
There are substantial individual differences in language development over time. Some children show evenly distributed growth in vocabulary, while other children exhibit a vocabulary “burst” (Bates et al, 1992). Just as physical development has a range of normativity (e.g., walking between 10 and 16 months) language development also has ranges for demonstrating communication skills, as outlined in the chart below.
Two–word phrases. Two-word phrases emerge as children master single words, then start to put them together in combination, “Me Cookie!” The developmental range for this is approximately 14 to 24 months with 20 months reported as the mean age (Bates, et al, 1992). There is a strong relationship between the development of words and word combinations, however there are variations in how children combine words to use language. In previous research, approximately 20% of children produced combinations of words with vocabularies under 50 words, while another 15% of children were not producing any word combinations even though their vocabularies were between 100 and 300 words (Bates, et al, 1992). Children typically add words to the two word combinations in phrases that first appear without grammar elements (Fenson et al, 1994). For example, when Imani said, “I no like it: go-fish (goldfish),” this demonstrates how a child uses short phrases to communicate their ideas without following grammatical rules.
Expressive Language
Refer to the early learning standards in your state to see the progression of expressive language skills. To review early learning language standards for receptive langauge in Wisconsin, visit Wisconsin Model Early Learning Standards.
Table 3.2.3 is from Virginia’s Early Learning & Development Standards (ELDS): Birth-Five Learning Guidelines. It is used with permission of the creators and is not an open education resource covered by a Creative Commons License.
DevelopmentalProgression
CLLD1.2 Using Vocabulary and Nonverbal Cues to Communicate
Early Infancy
0-8 months
Uses sound, inflection, and gestures to communicate needs, desires, or emotions (CLLD1.2a)
Cries, coos, babbles, and makes other sounds (CLLD1.2b)
Reaches and points to communicate (CLLD1.2c)
Later Infancy
6-14 months
Imitates actions of others as nonverbal communication (CLLD1.2d)
Makes vocal or non-vocal protests/demands (CLLD1.2e)
Vocalizes or gestures (e.g., pointing or using sign language) to communicate or to direct adult attention (CLLD1.2f)
Participates and initiates communication with family members or familiar others by using consistent sounds, gestures, and some words CLLD1.2g)
Early Toddler
12-24 Months
Repeats words heard in conversations (CLLD1.2h)
Makes simple one-word requests such as saying or signing “milk’ or “leche” when asking for a drink (CLLD1.2i)
Uses vocabulary for familiar items and events (e.g., hat, ball) (CLLD1.2j)
Begins to combine words, verbally or through signing (e.g., making requests through two- and multiword use/utterances such as “eat- or comer- ‘nana’/banana”) (CLLD1.2k)
Later Toddler
22-36 Months
Asks short questions and requests clarifications (CLLD1.2l)
Speaks in or signs short sentences, and some expanded sentences, and is understood by familiar peers or adults most of the time (CLLD1.2m)
Begins to use more diverse types of words, such as color and quantity words (all, some, more, and including numbers) (CLLD1.2n)
Early Preschool
34-48 Months
Begins to use complex sentences and ask simple questions (e.g., “After the gingerbread man ran, he swam on the fox’s back.”; “What happened to the gingerbread man?”) (CLLD1.2o)
Answers questions from unfamiliar people including some open-ended questions (CLLD1.2p)
Uses adjectives to describe people, objects, or environments (CLLD1.2q)
Begins to use prepositions (e.g., in, on) (CLLD1.2r)
Later Preschool
44-60 Months
Uses age-appropriate grammar in conversations and increasingly complex phrases and sentences (e.g., uses irregular past tense verbs such as came, ran, fell, broke, went, told, did) (CLLD1.2s)
Retells stories and events in sequence with multiple details (e.g., retells The Three Bears noting three chairs, three beds, etc.) (CLLD1.2u)
Social Language
Language cannot be considered outside of its social role as the purpose of language is to understand and be understood. This carries with it the weight of expectations, hopes, needs, and wishes. How we use sounds, gestures, and actions to communicate are social in nature. Conversations after all, must eventually have conversational partners. Vygotsky emphasized the transactional nature of language as he considered the influence of the social world on our cognitive development. Vygotsky emphasized that language was a tool for learning, rather than merely a reflection of what the child knows (1986). In fact, the conversational style of those nearest the child influences their language development as they mimic those around them. Adult behaviors such as expanding children’s utterances, and recasting and commenting on these utterances, is helpful for growth. Parents also differ in their conversational style, and so do siblings. Social language indicators include things like responding and turn-taking, or other signs of pragmatic language. Children’s language develops when they have experiences with conversational partners who engage with them and nurture their growth.
Learning and Engaging in Conversational Interactions
Table 3.2.4 “Learning and Engaging in Conversational Interactions”
Developmental Progression
CLLD1.3 Learning and Engaging in Conversational Interactions
Early Infancy
0-8 months
Responds and engages with an adult or older peer (CLLD1.3a)
Reacts to facial cues and eye contact (CLLD1.3b)
Engages by babbling (CLLD1.3c)
Later Infancy
6-14 months
Takes turns in “conversation” or vocal play with adults (CLLD1.3d)
Turns, smiles, and begins to speak when name is spoken (CLLD1.3e)
Begins to follow adult pointing or gaze to establish joint attention (e.g. looks across the room when adult points or gazes) (CLLD1.3f)
Early Toddler
12-24 Months
Responds with facial expressions, vocalizations, and/or gestures to engage in conversation with others (CLLD1.3g)
Later Toddler
22-36 Months
Uses simple verbal responses and nonverbal gestures (CLLD1.3h)
Uses words, gestures, signs, and phrases to converse with others (CLLD1.3i)
Begins to ask and respond to questions (CLLD1.3j)
Early Preschool
34-48 Months
Continues a conversation through several exchanges with or without adult help (CLLD1.3k)
Alternates between speaker/listener roles (CLLD1.3l)
Asks and responds to questions (CLLD1.3m)
Later Preschool
44-60 Months
Begins to match language to contexts (e.g., voice volume, inflection/rise and fall of voice in speaking, and parting/greeting rituals) (CLLD1.3n)
Engages in multiple back-and-forth conversations with adults in ways that can be goal directed (such as solving problems) (CLLD1.3o)
Asks and responds to questions with accurate information (CLLD1.4p)
Table 3.2.4 is from Virginia’s EarlyLearning & Development Standards (ELDS): Birth-Five Learning Guidelines. It is used with permission of the creators and is not an open education resource covered by a Creative Commons License.
References
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., & Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 21(1), 85–123. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900008680
Bates, E., Thal D., & Janowsky J.S. (1992). Early language development and its neural correlates. In S. J. Segalowitz & I. Rapin (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 7), (pp. 69–110). Elsevier.
Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders. Wiley.
Celci-Murcia, M., & Olshtain, E. (2001). Discourse and context in language teaching: A guide for language teachers. Cambridge University Press.
Conboy, B. T., & Kuhl, P.K. (2011). Impact of second-language experience in infancy: Brain measures of first- and second language speech perception. Developmental Science, 14, 242–248.
Fagan, Mary K. (2015). Why repetition? Repetitive babbling, auditory feedback, and cochlear implantation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 137, 125–136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.005
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., Tomasello, M., Mervis, C.B., & Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5). https://doi.org/10.2307/1166093
Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E. (2015). Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167
Karmiloff, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. The developing child series. Harvard University Press.
Koester, L. S., & Lahti-Harper, E. (2010). Mother-infant hearing status and intuitive parenting behaviors during the first 18 months. American Annals of the Deaf, 155(1), 5–18. https://doi.org/10.1353/aad.0.0134
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T., & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception and later language development: Implications for the “critical period.” Language Learning and Development, 1(3-4), 237–264. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15473341lld0103&4_2
Lipkind, D., Bemis, D. K., Marcus, G. F., & Tchernichovski, O. (2013). Analysis of the development of phonetic syntax in infant babbling. Protocol Exchange. https://doi.org/10.1038/protex.2013.057
Marno, H., Guellai, B., Vidal, Y., Franzoi, J., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (2016). Infants’ selectively pay attention to the information they receive from a native speaker of their language. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01150
Masataka, N. (1998). Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-month-old hearing infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(2), 241–246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.241
Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2005). Phonological development: Learning sounds and sound patterns. In J. B. Gleason (Ed)., The development of language (6th ed., pp. 39–61). Pearson.
Minai, U., Gustafson, K., Fiorentino, R., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. (2017). Fetal rhythm-based language discrimination: A biomagnetometry study. Neuroreport, 28(10), 561–564. https://doi:10.1097/WNR.0000000000000794
Stark R. E. (1986). Prespeech segmental feature development. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (Eds.) Studies in Language Acquisi-tion (149–173). Cambridge University Press.
Vihman, M. M., & Greenlee, M. (1987). Individual differences in phonological development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 30(4), 503–521. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3004.503