16.4: Morale Development
- Page ID
- 225530
\( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)
\( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)
\( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)
( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)
\( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)
\( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)
\( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\)
\( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)
\( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\)
\( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)
\( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\)
\( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)
\( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\)
\( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)
\( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\)
\( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)
\( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\)
\( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\AA}{\unicode[.8,0]{x212B}}\)
\( \newcommand{\vectorA}[1]{\vec{#1}} % arrow\)
\( \newcommand{\vectorAt}[1]{\vec{\text{#1}}} % arrow\)
\( \newcommand{\vectorB}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)
\( \newcommand{\vectorC}[1]{\textbf{#1}} \)
\( \newcommand{\vectorD}[1]{\overrightarrow{#1}} \)
\( \newcommand{\vectorDt}[1]{\overrightarrow{\text{#1}}} \)
\( \newcommand{\vectE}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash{\mathbf {#1}}}} \)
\( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)
\( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)
\(\newcommand{\avec}{\mathbf a}\) \(\newcommand{\bvec}{\mathbf b}\) \(\newcommand{\cvec}{\mathbf c}\) \(\newcommand{\dvec}{\mathbf d}\) \(\newcommand{\dtil}{\widetilde{\mathbf d}}\) \(\newcommand{\evec}{\mathbf e}\) \(\newcommand{\fvec}{\mathbf f}\) \(\newcommand{\nvec}{\mathbf n}\) \(\newcommand{\pvec}{\mathbf p}\) \(\newcommand{\qvec}{\mathbf q}\) \(\newcommand{\svec}{\mathbf s}\) \(\newcommand{\tvec}{\mathbf t}\) \(\newcommand{\uvec}{\mathbf u}\) \(\newcommand{\vvec}{\mathbf v}\) \(\newcommand{\wvec}{\mathbf w}\) \(\newcommand{\xvec}{\mathbf x}\) \(\newcommand{\yvec}{\mathbf y}\) \(\newcommand{\zvec}{\mathbf z}\) \(\newcommand{\rvec}{\mathbf r}\) \(\newcommand{\mvec}{\mathbf m}\) \(\newcommand{\zerovec}{\mathbf 0}\) \(\newcommand{\onevec}{\mathbf 1}\) \(\newcommand{\real}{\mathbb R}\) \(\newcommand{\twovec}[2]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\ctwovec}[2]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\threevec}[3]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\cthreevec}[3]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\fourvec}[4]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\cfourvec}[4]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\fivevec}[5]{\left[\begin{array}{r}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \\ #5 \\ \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\cfivevec}[5]{\left[\begin{array}{c}#1 \\ #2 \\ #3 \\ #4 \\ #5 \\ \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\mattwo}[4]{\left[\begin{array}{rr}#1 \amp #2 \\ #3 \amp #4 \\ \end{array}\right]}\) \(\newcommand{\laspan}[1]{\text{Span}\{#1\}}\) \(\newcommand{\bcal}{\cal B}\) \(\newcommand{\ccal}{\cal C}\) \(\newcommand{\scal}{\cal S}\) \(\newcommand{\wcal}{\cal W}\) \(\newcommand{\ecal}{\cal E}\) \(\newcommand{\coords}[2]{\left\{#1\right\}_{#2}}\) \(\newcommand{\gray}[1]{\color{gray}{#1}}\) \(\newcommand{\lgray}[1]{\color{lightgray}{#1}}\) \(\newcommand{\rank}{\operatorname{rank}}\) \(\newcommand{\row}{\text{Row}}\) \(\newcommand{\col}{\text{Col}}\) \(\renewcommand{\row}{\text{Row}}\) \(\newcommand{\nul}{\text{Nul}}\) \(\newcommand{\var}{\text{Var}}\) \(\newcommand{\corr}{\text{corr}}\) \(\newcommand{\len}[1]{\left|#1\right|}\) \(\newcommand{\bbar}{\overline{\bvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\bhat}{\widehat{\bvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\bperp}{\bvec^\perp}\) \(\newcommand{\xhat}{\widehat{\xvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\vhat}{\widehat{\vvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\uhat}{\widehat{\uvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\what}{\widehat{\wvec}}\) \(\newcommand{\Sighat}{\widehat{\Sigma}}\) \(\newcommand{\lt}{<}\) \(\newcommand{\gt}{>}\) \(\newcommand{\amp}{&}\) \(\definecolor{fillinmathshade}{gray}{0.9}\)- Define moral development and explain how it is influenced by social, emotional, and cognitive factors in early childhood.
- Describe how parenting style, peer interaction, and cultural values shape children’s moral understanding and behavior.
- Explain the role of empathy, conscience, and emotional regulation in guiding moral actions.
- Distinguish between moral rules and social conventions.
- D escribe how cognitive development supports moral reasoning and moral identity.
Role of the Environment
The environment plays a crucial role in shaping children's moral values and behaviors. Social interactions with parents, caregivers, teachers, and peers provide the foundation for understanding moral expectations and internalizing ethical standards.
- Warm, responsive parenting fosters a mutually responsive relationship, where children are motivated to follow moral guidelines out of trust and respect rather than fear.
- Positive discipline techniques, such as reasoning and explanation, help children internalize moral principles, whereas harsh punishment can lead to compliance out of fear rather than accurate understanding (Gershoff et al., 2018).
- Interactions with peers provide opportunities to practice fairness, cooperation, and perspective-taking.
- Through play, children learn to navigate conflicts, negotiate rules, and develop a sense of justice (Killen & Smetana, 2015).
- Moral lessons often emerge naturally in social interactions, such as when a child realizes that taking turns leads to more enjoyable play experiences.
- Additionally, moral values are shaped by cultural beliefs and societal norms.
- Some cultures emphasize collective responsibility and obedience, while others prioritize individual autonomy and fairness (Rogoff, 2003). Children absorb these cultural norms through interactions with their family, schools, and media, which shape their moral reasoning and behaviors.
Role of Emotional Development
Emotional development plays a fundamental role in moral behavior. Children’s ability to regulate emotions, experience empathy, and develop a sense of guilt or pride influences their moral decisions and actions. Empathy—the ability to understand and share another’s emotions—drives helping, sharing, and comforting behaviors (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010). By age three or four, children begin to show genuine concern for others and engage in simple acts of kindness, such as offering a toy to a crying friend.
The development of empathy is linked to secure attachments with caregivers and exposure to emotionally supportive environments. The emergence of conscience lays a crucial foundation for moral behavior. Children begin to internalize moral values and feel guilt or discomfort when they violate these standards (Thompson, 2012). This self-regulation is crucial for moral development, as it motivates children to act following moral principles even when no one is watching. Moral dilemmas often involve balancing selfish impulses with ethical choices. For example, resisting the urge to grab a toy from a peer requires effortful control—a self-regulation skill that develops during early childhood (Kochanska et al., 2015). Children with strong emotional regulation are better able to manage frustration and make moral choices based on fairness rather than immediate gratification.

Role of Cognitive Development
Cognitive development enables children to comprehend moral principles, fairness, and intentions. As children’s thinking skills mature, so does their ability to reason about moral issues. Young children begin to differentiate between moral rules (e.g., "Hitting is wrong") and social conventions (e.g., "We eat with a spoon"). By the age of four or five, children recognize that moral rules are universally important, whereas social conventions vary across cultures (Turiel, 2006). This distinction helps children understand why certain behaviors are wrong, regardless of the context.
As children’s theory of mind develops, they become better at recognizing others’ thoughts and feelings (Wellman, 2011). This ability allows them to engage in more complex moral reasoning, such as considering intentions behind actions. For example, a preschooler may understand that accidentally knocking over a block tower is different from deliberately destroying it. By the end of the preschool years, children begin to form a moral self-concept—an understanding of themselves as good or bad based on their actions (Thompson, 2012). Statements like “I’m a good helper” reflect early moral identity formation. Encouraging children to see themselves as kind and fair individuals strengthens their intrinsic motivation to act morally.
References, Contributors and Attributions
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 495-525.
Gershoff, E. T., Goodman, G. S., Miller-Perrin, C. L., Holden, G. W., Jackson, M. C., & Kazdin, A. E. (2018). The strength of the evidence against physical punishment of children and its implications for parents, psychologists, and policymakers. American Psychologist, 73(5), 626-638.
Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2015). Handbook of moral development. Routledge.
Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 339-351.
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., Boldt, L. J., & Yoon, J. E. (2015). Children's callous-unemotional traits moderate links between their positive relationships with parents at preschool age and externalizing behavior problems at early school age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(5), 576-584.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press.
Thompson, R. A. (2012). Whither the preconventional child? Toward a life‐span moral development theory. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 423-429.
Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. Handbook of child psychology, 3, 789-857.
Wellman, H. M. (2011). Developing a theory of mind. Psychology Press.